Pushkin. Angela, you don't know what a crossover is? Did you google it? Well? I did have to google it. The Simpsons did a crossover with twenty four. That's amazing. I think I get it, though, it's like the Dorito's Taco Bell mashup thing. The most horrifying new one of these is Hines just did this crazy new thing where they're doing all these weird Mayo ketchups and stuff. But
they did Oreo Mayo ketchup. What sounds terrible, But this will not be terrible because today we are doing a cognitive science mashup of the two best cognitive science podcasts out there, Let's be Real, Laurie Santo's from The Happiness Lab, along with Angela Duckworth from No Stupid Questions, and we are using this No Stupid Questions format that my usual partner, Stephen Dubner, and I like so much, which is that one of us asked a question of the other and
then we just have a rambling conversation. I'm in for the rambling conver station. That's usually what we have together when we meet up. Angela, let's do it. Thank the Lord that you're here, God, but I'm Angela Duckworth and this is Laurie Santos and you're listening to No Stupid Questions meets the Happiness Lab today on the show. Why do we mirror the accents and mannerisms of the people we meet? Monkey A observes monkey be do something different
with a banana. I don't know if that's stereotyping. They do like bananas. Totally true. Also, why can happiness seem so elusive even when things are going well? Holy Schmoley, this is the best life I could possibly living. Why am I not at ten out of ten on happiness? I think you have the first question for me? Correct? I do? And this comes from a listener named Sabika Shavan who hails from Qatar and is a graduate student there.
This question is as follows. I have a question about mimicking the mannerisms of people we meet, especially in our multicultural environments. Often in conversations with people with very market accents different from my own, I find my own accent taking on nuances of theirs or interjecting typical expressions from their language culture. Is this a typically observed behavior? And on the flip side, are there many whose accents and mannerisms never change regardless of who they speak to, and
a reason why some do and some don't. Laurie, I love this question. I am so vibing with it, Sabika. That is me. I spent two years in Oxford and I ended up speaking almost involuntarily with this faux British posh accent. And I am from southern New Jersey, so I should not be doing that. Does this happen to
you too? Oh my gosh. I moved to the UK when I was in graduate school, and I too took on not just like a British accent, the most painful British mannerisms, like what like if you told me something shocking, you did like, oh my gosh, Lorie, I just went to the pub and I had like eight points. I would respond to that with the phrase you walk. And I brought that back to the US for like a year,
and my friends were like, stop. It'd be one thing if you just talked with a fake British accent would be bad enough, but the fact that you're you wall all the time just terrible. Wait, you knew this and you still did it right. It wasn't an affectation that you were doing in an iranic Brooklyn way. Yeah, and I think you know this angela about me. But I grew up in New Bedford, Massachusetts, and had what, to
the uninitiated would sound like a terrible Boston accent. You were from Boston, you'd know it was a new Beefit accent, just like Pokya kad havid yad. Basically, I couldn't say are, to the point that I actually lost a research assistant position my freshman year in college. So I started out my freshman year working in Steve Coslin's lab with Kevin Oxner,
who's a professor at Columbia. He was doing some study where he just needed a female voice to say letters, and so I started recording these letters like hey b C. And then I got to q awe and he was like, well, you can't say awe. You have to say R. I was physically incapable of doing that except sounding like a pirate. I was like ah, and he was like, no, it's just our anyway. So now you notice that in my
perfect podcast speak, I can say are. But the reason I think that happened was that I ended up my freshman year being paired with a roommate from New Orleans, who also had an incredibly thick, this time Southern accent. Somehow, again perfectly unconsciously, we took on each other's vocal cadences to the point that by our senior year. This is back when your college room would have a single phone that someone would call and you wouldn't know who had
picked up except by their voice. And in fact, people couldn't tell me and my roommate Catherine apart by our senior year because our voices had converged so much. You took her rs. Did she lose hers? She stopped saying y'all as much. In fact, we both picked up another expression from our Pittsburgh roommate, which was yins. I like that because you guys is apparently an offensive, gender, presumptuous, possibly hostile sounding appellation. So my own students tell me,
I think ian's and y'all are better. But the beauty is we just do this naturally. In fact, this is an evolved part of human cognition. Researchers call this behavioral contagion. This is the kind of thing that you see in animals, you know, classic cases. If you watch fish, they tend to school around and it looks like they're all kind of copying each other's behavior. There's some lovely work by this guy Ian Cousin, who does all these detailed mathematical
network analyses of how fish school around. But the upshot is they're just soaking up each other's behavior quite naturally. How do you know that they are really mimicking each other as opposed to all responding to the same little piece of floating kelp. He does these incredibly detailed Matthew things that I'm not going to be able to pull
up on the fly. He can actually do some predictive coding based on one fish's behavior about what's going to happen with the schooling, and so it really does seem to be behavioral contagion. But you don't need to look to fish. This is something that we do quite naturally. One of the most famous examples of this came from my colleague here at Yale, John Barge and his colleague
Tanya Chartrand. They found out about this effect that they called the chameleon effect, well labeled because it's cases where people just chameleonly copy other people's behavior. What's an example. They had subjects get interviewed by a scary experimenter. So there's just like, high status person, do you think you're being interviewed by? And what the experimenter did, unbeknownst to the subjects was just occasionally take on strange movements, so she would touch her face or put her arm in
a particular way, or cross her legs. And what you find is that as they're being interviewed, they unconsciously copy all these behaviors. So as the experimenters touching her face, they touched their face more. As she's foldening her legs, they pull their legs more, again totally unconsciously. And their later work shows that this happens more in the high status direction, so you're more likely to unconsciously copy the high status people, maybe just because you're watching them more. Honestly.
This is the classic work of Albandura also right, where the little children who watch an adult take a toy like a bobo doll and beat it with a hammer. Then when entering a room with a bobo doll just like they saw, they will walk over and start beating it with a hammer, whereas Bandara points out, they don't do that in a control condition where they have not seen an adult model this, So I guess the question I'm asking is, is the phenomenon that you're describing different
from modeling or basically the same. It's probably the mechanism that leads to this kind of stuff. I mean, you know this well. In cognitive science, we often don't know the basic mechanisms that lead to other stuff down the line. And there's lots of hypotheses that things like behavioral contagient lead to lots of nasty stuff. In fact, mandura is
about people beating up a bobo doll. But there's evidence from people like Francesca Gino and Dan Arielli that behavioral contagient can lead to some truly unethical behavior in some contexts lying, cheating, stealing, and worse. Exactly, the Dan Ailee francescan you know cheating study is fantastic. So they bring these subjects into the lab college students and give them a super super hard math test. It's basically impossible. And so if you're a subject, you're experience and like, oh
my god, this is terrible. I'm never going to finish this. And then you watch one subject raise their hand immediately like two seconds into the experiment and say, yeah, I'm done. What can I do. And the experiments like, if you're done well, you can just shred your answers so no one sees them and will pay you. And so if you're the subject, you think you wait a minute, They're not even going to check that I did them. They're
just going to shred it. And what Arielli and colleagues find is you're more likely to cheat on these problems if you see somebody else cheating. But the neat thing is that it's not just if you see somebody else cheating, it actually has to be somebody that relates to you. And so they manipulated this in a cute way. They're doing this stuff at Duke University. So the Duke students are there in this study, and the person that raises their hand and cheats is in a Duke University sweatshirt. Now,
all of a sudden, cheating goes up a lot. But if the person's in an UNC sweatshirt, who are like the lose from the other school, Now all of a sudden people are like, oh my god, I'm not going to cheat, and it actually reinforces moral behavior. And so this is another thing we know about behavioral contagion, which is kind of weird. We're more likely to contagiously pick up on the behaviors of people who we see as our in group members, who we see as high status,
who we pay attention to. So I wanted to ask you what you thought about mirror neurons. I, as more or as an outsiders as literature, have only read articles about these specialized neurons that if I see you doing a particular action, they are lighting up in my brain as if I were doing the same action. So A, is that an accurate description of mirror neurons? And B what up with mirror neurons? Lorie, I'm kind of not
a fan of mirror neurons. I'll be totally honest. There's a lot of hype a brown mirror neurons, but what they actually do might not be as cool as we sometimes think. Basically, these were discovered in monkeys in a very famous set of experiments back in the early nineties where monkeys were watching humans engaging in these actions, and areas of the monkey motor cortex the spot that would
fire if they were grabbing for something. When they are watching these humans grabbing for something, they tended to fire it. So it seems super cool, like, oh my gosh, the same neuron in me that fires when I reach fires when I see you reach. Maybe neurons are the code for empathy. Maybe these neurons are the seat of our perspective taking all the stuff. What we know about them is they only exist in motor cortex, so it's for very specific motoric movements like grabbing and reaching. There's a
couple mirror neurons in other spots. There's some that might be in the tension region, so for igs turning and stuff like that, but not as rich as you'd think. And I think there is some argument that human beings are unique in their ability to learn through observation, whereas a dog or even a chimpanzee can't do it, or at least can't do it as well, Which is kind of right, because the mirror neurons were mostly found in monkeys. In terms of learning by observation, animals do do that,
but what they don't learn by is imitation. Like I see you behave in this very specific sequence, and I copy all of those very mechanical behaviors perfectly. That's literally what monkeys don't do. Wait, what do monkeys do? So monkey a observes monkey bee do something different with a banana. I don't know if that's stereotyping. They do like bananas. Totally true. Okay, good, I'm glad that holds up. Anyway, What does happen if it's not imitation, what is it?
Here's one study that looked at this. This is not with monkeys, but with chimpanzees. For our listeners out there, pet peeve of people who work with primates monkeys, not chimpanzees. Chimpanzees actually eat monkeys, so totally different. What it's like saying a human is a tuna fish sandwich or something. So with chimpanzees, they have this task where there's a bunch of food outside of some enclosure and chimp has
to use a tool to get it. They give a chimp a rake basically where you could try to use a rake with the times down like we'd normally rake leaves, but if they're tiny pieces of food, that works sort of but not super well because the food goes through the times. Whereas if you flip the rake over and you have that part of the rake that's flat, you
can scoop the food up more effectively. So they show kids this behavior, and what you find is the kid will copy whatever the human does, whereas if you do the same thing with chimpanzees, they don't necessarily fully copy what the human does. They realize like, oh, I can use a rake to try to get the food, and then they try and error it. So they're kind of copying the fact that you're using this tool and you can do it. But what they're not copying is the
perfect actions that go with it. It sounds smarter, doesn't It sound a little bit more evolved as it were? It is smarter. Yeah. In fact, there's a wonderful bias that is perhaps human unique bias. We have some evidence that you don't see it in primates or in dogs, which is called overimitation, this idea that we imitate too much. If you see somebody doing something that's inefficient or in the case of this Arielli study we talked about bad
or like immoral, you inadvertently copy it. Anyway. Have you read the studies of Christine Leguere, this developmental psychology work on children imitating others? Yeah, and she finds with over imitation, but part of it seems to be automatic, but part of it seems to be because this kind of behavioral contigient is our way of showing hey, I'm in the group like you. And this gets to another way that you could think about switching accents in particular, this idea
of code switching. So code switching is if you're a member of a minority group and you're in a majority group situation, you sort of switch your behavior around to match what the majority group is doing, which is sometimes considered not a great thing, but arguably, as you're pointing out, is adaptive totally. If I look back at my own accent, switching, my new beefit accent wasn't going to necessarily work super well in IVY League classes. That wasn't the way these
high status, higher class people talked. It's no secret that my accent switched more towards a IVY League vernacular English. Right, So we were both in England, which was a higher status accent, one could argue, certainly than my native South Jersey. So I start speaking a faux British accent. But if a British scholar for whatever reason, came to southern New Jersey and had to spend a summer down the shore, they would not adopt the local vernacular right, That would
be the prediction because of the status. Status is part of it, for sure, but I think also just functionally getting the inside scoop and seeming like you belong and you're like an insider at that place. So my prediction is brit might do it less in southern New Jersey than this Southern New Jersey, or would do it in
the UK, but they would to a certain extent. And this is the reason that again I have lost sadly at my New Bedford accent until I go back to New Bedford for a couple of days and then I all of a sudden sound like I've been there my whole life. I want to hear a New Bedford happiness lab. I think you should do it full on and maybe you could record it there another time it happens. Maybe you got this too when you were in the UK. Is when I'm drunk, those more automatic accents come back.
It's weird. I haven't been drunk since I was eighteen. But that is probably a different question. So I'll have to actually get data on that and come back to you. There's an experiment we could do. Angela. Okay, so now I get to ask a question right, which is so cool. We don't normally do this on the Happiness Lab. You need another person to be hanging out with, that's true, You're welcome anytime on Happiness Lab. Angela, thank you. But
here's question number two. Amelia asks why is it that so many people are restless or unsatisfied even in terrific or satisfying circumstances. Her context is that she's in a really lovely place with lots of supportive and happy colleagues. She's exceeded all these expectations she's had for herself professionally, but still finds herself looking around at other opportunities, kind of feeling unhappy, thinking she should switching. And then she goes on to ask why can't I just wait it out?
Why the need for change? Maybe at some level people don't want to be happy? What is the deal? Scientifically? This is such a great question. I think it is timely because, as we both were very sad to know a Dener, the scientist who arguably more than anyone, put the scientific study of happiness on the map. He passed away very recently, So I feel like this question is a way also for us to honor the great Ed Dener, who is amazing and who stuff we talk about all
the time on the Happiness Lab. So I will begin by saying that I had this experience myself. I remember when I was I think eighteen years old writing to Dear Abbey, saying how unhappy I was. There were extending circumstances. Mostly I was an adolescent. So that's partly your job as adolescent to be unhappy with your circumstances. Wait, wait, wait, wait, time out. You actually wrote to Dear Abbey, legit wrote
to Dear Abbey. Yes, who was the nineteen eighties. So I wrote a letter, put it in an envelope, licked it, sealed it, put a stamp on it, and mailed it away. And what my letter said was more or less that I felt like I had a perfect life. I had just gotten into college, I know, was going to Harvard, so I was getting lots of praise from my Asian family. And my boyfriend at the time was somebody I thought I would spend maybe the rest of my life with. And I had a wonderful best friend, and all these
great things were happening to me. And there was this contrast from the objective awesomeness of my life and my unst I was just like, I'm not happy, Abby, what's wrong with me? What did Abby say? She said to go see a therapist. Go see a therapist. Art for excellent words of advice. But I was very disappointed at
the time. And you know what, Laurie, I wish I had listened to her, because it took me a couple more decades to realize that I'm in the state of mind where I'm so unhappy I need to write a letter to a stranger to ask them for help. Then, really what I should do? Let's go see a therapist.
But there are times where you just look around at your life in any objective sense, you realize, holy shmoley, this is the best life I could possibly be living, and you have this gnawing sense of dissatisfactor, like, why am I not a ten out of ten on happiness? Now? I want to start our scientific discussion of this with this very famous idea of the hedonic treadmill, which I know you've probably already discussed on your podcast. Am I write on that? Yeah? We had a fantastic guest on
to talk about the hedonic treadmill, Clay Cockerell. He's a wealth psychologist, so he's a mental health professional for the point zero zero one percent. First off, that's telling right that we have to have mental health professionals for the point zero zero one percent. You think these people would be like, they're not so happy that they're like, no,
I'm good. Yeah, they need him, and the problems he sees and his patients are just I mean, if you're not in the point zero zero one percent, you kind of get a little bit schadenfreude because they're things like I don't know where to park my yacht and you're like, well, dude, maybe if you don't have a yacht. But the point is, these ostensibly objectively terrific circumstances don't always feel terrific, and that is the hedonic treadmill. We kind of just get
used to stuff. So if you have something objectively awesome happen, you notice and you feel that it is good, and it affects your happiness for a short while, but then you kind of just get used to it. And that's the idea of treadmill. You like, keep running and running, you stay in the same place. But I think we would both agree that there is to some extent, a phenomenon by which, through either things that we do intentionally or unconsciously, we do come back from either extreme like
too happy or the opposite. The flip side is that we also get used to circumstances that are pretty awful. They don't continue to affect our psychology as bad as when they first happen. So you break up or you lose a job, those things for a while and they feel like they're going to suck forever. I think that's
one of the fascinating things about emotions. When you're in the middle of one, like when you're anxious or lonely or extremely sad, you can't really see around the corner, even if intellectually you realize like, oh, yeah, I've been in this kind of place before and I've seen things get better. But doesn't feel that way in any visceral sense.
But though we would agree with that, I think one of the nuances here that is important to underscore is that the returning to the set point isn't always exactly to where you were before. So the famous nineteen seventy eight study of accident victims who became paraplegics, it's often described as follow them long enough, you see that they come back to where they were before their accident, but sadly not quite. Yes, they adapt to donically, but not
all the way back on average to where they were before. Yeah, and there's a few cases like that where adaptation isn't perfect. I think another one is in the context of unemployment. That's another case where you go down a little bit. Actually, one that's the opposite is divorce. You have a hit to your happiness when you first get divorced, but you actually pop up past baseline. This is the thing about
these happiness set points. They're not perfect. Sometimes you go at tency bit down or a tency bit up in the good cases. But the point is that moves. It's not like this person's going to break up with me and I'm stuck there forever. It has to move. Let's talk about coming down from the highs people do. At least a lot of us walk around basically shooting for the ten out of ten, Like why can't I be
a ten out of ten every day? Is there something we can say about the adaptiveness of not living life at the extreme end of like everything is great when you think about the extreme end. This reminds me not of a scientific tip but a philosophical one. Aristotle thought that virtue was living in the middle. You know, if you're shooting to be brave, you don't want to be like the bravest dude as such that you're reckless, But you also don't want to be cowardly, And so there's
something to be said for this with happiness too. Happiness is going to be elusive if you're constantly analyzing do I have it yet? Do I have yet? Do I have it yet? We really want to get to a point where we're feeling grateful, noticing the good stuff in our lives, doing everything we can to savor what we have. But pushing, pushing, pushing might not necessarily be the best
thing for your happiness anyway. I have long pondered this Aristotle golden mean idea, in part because the things I study, like self control or grit, people always ask can you be too self controlled? What if you're too gritty? What's the dark side of excessive grit? And when I think
about what Aristotle saying, I'm a little confused. What is the deeper reason why something in between the extremes is, as a rule better not just in the case of bravery and cowardice, but as a general truth about human nature. And I wonder whether there is some cost to being at the ten out of ten which makes us not want to be there all the time. Well, I think one is if you were always at a ten out of ten and you never change, you wouldn't notice any change.
And I think this actually gets to Amelia's question. She's asking why the need for change. The need for change is that we don't notice our absolute objective status. We only notice when we change from it. People who live in southern California don't appreciate the weather because it's just
perfect all the time. But when you live in the Northeast, you get enough sucky days that all of a sudden, when it's sunny out, you're like, oh my gosh, it's sunny in eighty Thank you, you know, whatever divinity you're praying to. The other thing is, I think you're totally right on the cost. Sometimes, if you're pushing happiness too much, that can be costly, and I think we see that
in the context of clinical disorders like mania. Those people would report I'm ten out of ten on a happiness scale, but they're gambling and wrecking their car and hurting their family and things like that. And so I think the Aristotle might not have been perfect with the middle Road, but he was onto something and something I think he was onto. It's most relevant to Amelia gets back to this idea of the power of change. When we're just
consistently we kind of don't notice it. The consequence of that is what Danny Counterman and Amos Tversky referred to as diminishing sensitivity. We can get small changes that objectively feel good, but we just don't notice them, which is sort of sad. The example I give my students is I try to be hit right, like I try to know what a crossover is and know all the new songs. And there's this DJ College song called All I Do is Win. Do you know this song? No, of course
I don't sell it. It's like all I do is win, win, win, No matter what the ideas, he just wins all the time. And I tell my students that is like a crappy way to live a life, because if you're literally winning all the time, you don't actually notice the subtle changes. What is the optimal design, then, of a good life? Maybe it's just ninety nine great days and one really bad one to make you appreciate the rest of the ninety nine. What do you think? I think about this
one a lot. What you want to do is maximize the change somehow, and it's optimal if that change is going in a positive direction, but you actually want it to go down sometimes because another feature of this diminishing sensitivity I mentioned Danny Koneman and Amos diversity. It comes from their famous idea about prospect theory, which is this idea that we don't evaluate prospects or things in our lives in terms of absolute values. We've recognized them and
represent them in terms of changes. You know, if I was like Angela, right now, the happiness Lab is going to give you one million dollars, you'd be like, that's amazing. But if I was like, right now, the happiness Lab is going to give you two million dollars, I mean, that's better, but you're not like twice as happy, and
so that is diminishing sensitivity, and that sucks. It means for you to get that extra happiness benefit from the extra million in the two million, you would have wanted to go back to baseline first, so it feels like two separate gains instead of one big gain. That's another
reason not to obsess about being at ten exactly. You think going from nine to ten is going to be just as good as going from eight to nine or seven to eight, But according to diminishing returns, like, eh, it's better, not much better as it was to go from seven to eight. That gives us some hints about how to do it better. Right, So one is split your gains. You don't want two million at once, you want one million and then come back a couple months
later and get another million. This is something I actually try to do. How how do you do it? Sometimes my husband and I will have a date night and we're like, all right, we're gonna see the movie we really wanted and get the dinner we really wanted and get ice cream too. And it's like, wait, let's do the nice dinner and then do the ice cream tomorrow. A really stupid way I do this is sometimes when
I'm buying stuff. This is not very ecologically savvy, so maybe this is not helping with my climate change goals, But you know what it's like, you get the package of a bunch of stuff, that you bought on Amazon. It's not as fun as if you got the shirt one day and then the next day you got the shoes and you're like, oh yeah, so you split your gains. I have a proposal that may or may not have
as severe consequences getting two Amazon packages. You know, vacations are something that I don't know how to take very well. But according to the principles we've been discussing, rather than taking seven days off and cramming in all of your dinners out and your extra desserts and your walks around whatever city you're in with ice coffe, that would be my preferred vacation. Why don't we have seven three day weekends. I think that that could result in a massive global
gain in happiness without any obvious downside. I love it. My other tip on this, I don't know if you like Hostess cupcakes. I'm from Billions. We have Tasty cakes. Oh. I think tasty cakes are similar, But the key to the Hostess cupcake is that you get two of them. Like, Hostess could have made that much chocolate cakiness in a single big cupcake, but if you got that cupcake, you just plowed through it. They had the insight to break
those up, and like what happens? Because you eat the first one, you wait, and then you come back to it. You're kind of at baseline again. You get more happiness. Wow, you think that the Hostess people really had behavioral science stuff. They read prospect theory. They're like, wait a minute, this is why people like the mini black and white cookies
better than the one huge black and white cookie. You can pause in between them, and you go back to happiness baseline, no cookie, and then you're like, oh my gosh, another cookie, and then spike backup. I don't know how many people, by the way, eat the one Hostess or Tasty cake. In the Tasty Cake version, there's no white squirly line across the top, but it's basically the same cupcake.
But there's two, right, there is two. And I do think spacing out our gains could be helpful, just as you recommend, and maybe just reframing the inevitable bad days as like, I mean, here's a trivial example. Last night I made Casha, you know, the buckwheat thing. Oh yeah, I followed the recipe to the letter because had a friend. She's like, I'm gonna call my grandmother, We're going to
get this exactly right. And then I left the pot on the stove, not even thinking, and it just burned to a crisp and it was horrible and both mushy and burned at the same time, which I didn't think was physically possible. So that was a bad experience. I grieved a bit. But maybe if I reframe that as hooray for the burned casha, now to make the next non burnt batch all that much more delicious and appreciate it totally. And in fact, this gets back to a
different form of ancient wisdom. This was exactly the strategy that the Stoics had. So the Stoics thought you should every morning do what they call negative visualization. You wake up and you say, my Kasha is going to get burned, my husband's gonna leave me, I'm going to lose my job, I'm gonna trip and break my leg. You don't ruminate on that forever, but you do that as little kind of five to him in meditation, and you go to your day and you're like, oh my gosh, my Kasha
didn't burn today. So the Stoics were really into this idea that you don't necessarily have to have the change to notice the change. You could just imagine the change, and it gives you a lot of gratitude for the stuff you have. One technique I used in some of my talkses I look out in the audience and I say, all of you people who have kids, imagine whatever the last time you saw them was, that was the last time. It's over. You're never going to see them again. Any ideas.
The next time you hug your kids, you're going to hug them much more tightly. You didn't have to have a horrible thing happen to them. The reference point didn't have to change in a bad way for you get the appreciation. I had a shutter. I just had to say, Laurie, that was rough. But now you're going to be so nice to your kids today. Yeah, even if they're annoying, you'd be like, but I'm so happy they're alive. Like, thank the Lord that you're here. Okay, you have given
us one thing you could do. You could wake up and think of three bad things and they are just imaginary, and then the whole rest of your day is going to go better. But I recall the study that you and I did wake up and think of three good things, right, the classic gratitude exercise. These are opposite recommendations. So should people wake up and think of three good things or should they wake up and think of three bad things?
I'm going to vote for the three good things. I did this as I usually do this morning, and I actually thought about the Kasha. Thank god the house didn't burn down because I did discover the pot of burning buckwheat in time to prevent a fire. Yay. And then I thought of a couple of other things. My daughter got home safely. I really love this collaborator and built in is a contrast to the counterfactual, like my could be a jerk, but they're not, and my daughter are
goold have not gotten home safely. So maybe the stoics had a good idea, but I think it's improved upon by this much more positive experience of thinking about three good things. To be fair, I think that's what the stoics mean. They don't mean like, oh my god, my house is going to burn out. They think you should do that because immediately afterwards you're going to think about
the positive thing too. You're gonna be grateful for your kids, leaving the stuff on the floor because you had that moment of thinking about what it could be like to not have kids at all. I think naturally, in the way the stokes are talking about them, they focus on the negative side, but they're hoping you're going to get
to the blessings really fast. And I think the negative side is important when you're feeling really down, Like the example of breaking your like because I'm clumpsy, this actually happens to me with reasonable frequency, Like I recently broke my knee. You literally mean this happens too with frequency. You injure yourself in a serious way. Yeah, this was the second time I'd broken the same kneecap when I fell on it. Oh my god, Laurie, it's terrible. Yeah,
I was like, whoa is me? I broke my knee cap? This sucks. And then I actually went back to the stoics because I know I needed hardcore people who are going to help me with this. And I read a book by this current practicing stoic, Bill Urvine, and he went through like, let's talk about some cases that you could have. He's like, you could be a shut in. These are people who have some sort of accident happen who are fully conscious but so paralyzed that they can't
move any part of their body. They have to like blink an eye to communicate with people. And I was like, okay, well, at least I don't have that. But sometimes if you get the right negative visualization, you're like, wait a minute, I can actually be grateful for the broken knee too, because at least it's not X, Y and Z. And I think this is a nice way to solve Amelia's problem. You don't necessarily have to get the change from your real actions. You can make your current reference points seem
good just through these imaginations. Do you think that would change Amelia's set point? Do you think that if she chronically were comparing her pretty awesome life. She says, she is a lovely department. She's doing really well. If you regularly did these mental counterfactuals, that she would be enduringly happier.
You know, if every morning she could have the counterfactual of what if I didn't have this lovely, supportive job with my interesting colleagues, As she mentions, what if my colleagues suck that bumps up the appreciation you have, It breaks your heat on a adaptation. So I actually do think it would be kind of a nice strategy. I think we need Amelia to agree to be a pilot
subject in a study with only one subject. So, Laurie, you want Amelia to wake up every day for a week and what think of three bad things of the things she loves about her life and her job. Imagine that those weren't there. I would propose the second week be that she tried the three good things exercise, and after a month we could all get together and find
out which week was better. I want a third condition where she does both, where she imagines the bad thing and then thinks, oh my gosh, I am so lucky to have these colleagues because I think if you just do bad then it could be And to be fair to the stoics, that's not really what they meant. Okay, now we need six weeks of your life, Amelia, right, love it to be continued. The Happiness Lab is co
written and produced by Ryan Dilley. Our original music was composed by Zachary Silver, with additional scoring, mixing, and mastering by Evan Viola. That Happiness Lab is brought to you
by Pushkin Industries. Any doctor Laurie Santos No Stupid Questions as part of the Freakonomics Radio Network, which also includes Freakonomics Radio People I mostly Admire and Freakonomics MD. This episode was produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas for our Happiness Lab listeners who are new to No Stupid Questions. This is the time in the show where I do a quick fact check of the conversations. Early on in the episode, Laurie and Angela say that they're horrified by
the idea of heinz Oreo mayonnaise. I'm sure they will be thrilled to hear that this is not, in fact a real product. In June of twenty twenty one, the Instagram account doctor Photograph posted a convincing photo of may Oreo sauce that immediately went viral, but the image was later proven to be altered. For those who are disappointed that this crossover product doesn't actually exist, fret notot plenty
of other hind mashups are actually real. The company now produces Mayo Chup, a combination of mayonnaise and ketchup, Mayo must a mix of mayonnaise and mustard, and Cranch, a blend of Ketchup, and Ranch, among others. Later, Angela and Laurie discuss recently deceased psychologist Albert Bendora's seminal Bobo Doll experiment. I was unfamiliar with the concept of a bobo doll, and I surmised that many listeners would be as well.
I found that the toy isn't really a doll at all, but rather a large, inflatable plastic clown with a heavy, rounded bottom. When it's pushed over, the clown temporarily wobbles, but quickly bounces back to center, making it a perfect toy for children and adults to beat up in Bendora's experiments. Finally, Angela says that Tasty cakes, like Hostess cupcakes, come two to a package. Standard Tasty Cake boxes do include six packs of two cupcakes, but you can also opt for
a single package of three cupcakes. Either way, if you have the self restraint, you can still enjoy the happiness that comes with more dessert later that day. That's it for the fact check. No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher. Our staff includes Alison Craiglowe, Greg Rippin, James Foster, Joel Meyer, Trisha Bobita, Emma Terrell, Lyric Bowtitch, Jasmine Clinger and Jacob Clementi.