The Trouble With Zero - podcast episode cover

The Trouble With Zero

Jan 01, 202512 minEp. 1196
--:--
--:--
Listen in podcast apps:
Metacast
Spotify
Youtube
RSS

Episode description

Happy New Year, Short Wavers! What better time to contemplate the conundrum that is zero than this, the reset of the year? Zero is a fairly new concept in human history and even more recent as a number. It wasn't until around the 7th century that zero was being used as a number. That's when it showed up in the records of Indian mathematicians. Since then, zero has, at times, been met with some fear — at one point, the city of Florence, Italy banned the number.

Today, scientists seek to understand how much humans truly comprehend zero — and why it seems to be different from other numbers. That's how we ended up talking to science writer Yasemin Saplakoglu about the neuroscience of this number that means nothing.

Read more of Yasemin's reporting on zero for Quanta Magazine. Plus, check out our episode on why big numbers break our brains.

Thirst for more math episodes? Let us know what kind of stories you want to hear from us in 2025 by emailing [email protected]!

Listen to every episode of Short Wave sponsor-free and support our work at NPR by signing up for Short Wave+ at
plus.npr.org/shortwave.

Learn more about sponsor message choices: podcastchoices.com/adchoices

NPR Privacy Policy

Transcript

This message comes from Wondery Kids and the number one kid's science podcast, Wow in the World. Be wowed by fascinating companion audio stories that come exclusively with each Wow in the World STEM toy. Shop the full collection today at Amazon.com slash Wondery Kids. You're listening to Shortwave. From NPR. Hey, Sure Waivers, it's Regina Barber. And today is the first day of 2025. Happy New Year!

The new year is all about blank slates. New beginnings starting from scratch. And so we thought, what better time than now to focus on the number that signifies origin points. Literally starting from nothing. Zero. So, Zira was invented relatively late in history. It was first thought to be invented around like 2,500 years ago by Babylonian traders in ancient Mesopotamia, actually. That's Yasmin Soplakolu. She's a science writer at Quantum Magazine.

Back then, they used the symbol like two slanted wedges on clay tablets. But at the time, it wasn't a number yet. It was really used as a placeholder so that you can distinguish between different types of numbers like 20 or 250 or... And Yasmin says that this idea of a placeholder wasn't totally unique. The ancient Maya, for example, had a little shell symbol that they used in a similar way. But zero didn't really become a number on its own.

until around the 7th century. There were Indian mathematicians who came up with a couple of ways to use zero as a number, and they were the kind of first to figure out that zero could be. a digit just like the other numbers like one and two and three. After that, it kind of went out from India to the Arab world. And then, you know, in the 13th century, Feminacci actually picked up the idea during his travels in North Africa.

And he brought it back to medieval Europe, you know, along with the base 10 number system. But in medieval Europe, not everyone was thrilled about this concept of zero. People had difficulty with accepting it. It was kind of scary. People were confused by it. Some thought of it as like the devil's number that challenged like really deeply held ideas. And, you know.

Because of the influence of the church, like philosophers and theologians associated nothing with like chaos and disorder. One city, Florence, Italy, actually banned the number zero altogether.

It's a weird concept if you even think too deeply about it. It's like we're describing something that doesn't exist, right? We like see three chairs or we see... four birds and we can count those and they're you know physically there but we don't see zero birds or zero chairs we just know that they're absent but that is zero that's you know

An extra level of abstraction from the other kinds of numbers that we see around us all the time. And that abstraction actually makes it harder for our brains to process. So today on the show, the neuroscience of the number zero. How do humans think about the concept of nothing? How do we find out? And what does that mean for our brains? You're listening to Shortwave, the science podcast from NPR.

This message comes from Side Note by ASAP Science, a podcast about the science behind controversial subjects. Is marijuana addictive? How will the universe end? Hosts Mitch and Greg dissect a new topic every Wednesday on your favorite podcast app. Every weekday, Up First gives you the news you need to start your day. On the Sunday story from Up First, we slow down. We bring you the best reporting from NPR journalists around the world, all in one major story, 30 minutes or less.

Join me every Sunday on the Up First podcast to sit down with the biggest stories from NPR. Since the beginning of women's sports, there's been a struggle to define who qualifies for the women's category. Tested from NPR's Embedded podcast and CBC takes you inside that struggle. Listen to Tested, the series that was named one of the 10 best podcasts of 2024 by Apple, Vulture, and The New York Times. It's season 20 of NPR's Embedded podcast.

Okay, Yasmin, before we get into the complexity of zero, let's just start with the neuroscience of numbers in general. You've done some writing about this, like how our brains comprehend small numbers differently than large numbers. Can you tell us more about that? Right. So this is actually kind of cool. We have what are called number neurons in the brain. So there are neurons that are tuned to specific numbers. There are neurons that favor.

For example, the number 5. And there are other neurons that favor the number 7. And so on. So that means that... for the neurons that favor the number five, they'll fire more. Like if it sees five items on a table, it'll fire more than if it sees four or...

you know, six. It'll still fire for four and six, but less. And then even less for, you know, three and seven. Yeah, so these neurons are specifically like four... that number they like really like that number yeah yeah it's it's like kind of neat that we have all of these and there's a lot of questions surrounding it still like we don't know if there are neurons that fire for like a thousand five hundred and five

That seems like there would be a lot of neurons in the brain then for the various numbers. I'm sure there's some other kind of mechanism there. And yeah, there was this research recently that found that the brain actually analyzes small numbers different than it does larger numbers. So there's... weird boundary around the number four. There seems to be some sort of

double mechanism that's happening. So the brain processes numbers that are smaller than four in a more precise way than it does for numbers larger than four. Yeah, we did an episode about this like way back at the beginning of last year. And like how if you get above four, you're not counting. You're actually comparing. And when you're below four, that's when you're literally like your brain is counting. Yeah. So you're not counting anymore past four. So interesting, right? It's like.

I think about this all the time where it's like, why is it four? Like, why not five? But it's four. And when I reported that piece, a couple of experts were talking to me about how it's also... Weirdly related to working memory or awareness. So people can only really hold a certain number of objects in their awareness. And that's four.

So they think that maybe there's some sort of connection there between, you know, how we're processing numbers and working memory. Yeah, it's like phone numbers, right? We're in chunks of three and four to like... for us to remember. Same with like, you know, social security number and stuff like that. Right. So fairly recently, these like two researchers were like, hey, if zero special like in math and history, maybe and this is what you were saying, maybe it's special in neuroscience.

Maybe we think about it differently. One group looked at patients with epilepsy. They already had these like electrodes in their brain. So the researchers were able to see like how individual neurons were firing. Yeah. And the other one? The other group looked more at populations of neurons. kind of a larger scale. They used a magnetoencephalography scanner. That sounds right. That was impressive. Long word.

And basically that means that, you know, as the neurons fire, they generate voltages, which creates magnetic fields that the machine can detect. And by analyzing the magnetic fields, the researchers were able to kind of probe what the neurons were doing when the participants were prompted to think about zero. That's so cool.

So these researchers, they're looking at neurons either specifically or as a group, you know, firing. What did they find? Was there like a new discovery on how we think about zero? So they actually they were looking for something called the numerical distance effect, which is basically a phenomenon.

That occurs when the brain processes non-zero numbers. And it means that it can more easily distinguish numbers that are far apart from each other than those that are close together. So the brain has a little bit more difficulty distinguishing between. like seven and eight versus seven and 10, for example, or seven and 11. Got it. Okay. So the idea for both.

these groups was that we should see if zero also is part of this numerical distance effect, because if it is, then the brain might be seeing zero just as it does the other numbers, just the normal number. Just a normal number. Yeah. Yeah. Probably not. Kinda. What happened? Yeah. So the first group, the one that looked at a bigger scale, found that the brain... processes zero similarly to other numbers basically it puts zero at

the start of a mental number line, like it's before one. It showed the numerical distance effect, which is what they were looking for. So their conclusion was there's no difference in the way that the brain sees zero than the other numbers. zero in terms of both the digit zero and like zero objects um the second group also found that uh The brain puts zero before one on the mental number line, but they found subtle differences that still made zero special.

in the brain okay so for example they found that more neurons had zero as their preferred number than other small numbers that suggested to them that the brain might be representing you know this empty set with more accuracy than it does for other small quantities. But this was only true for quantity zero. For the digit zero, they did not find any difference. Like the brain saw the digit zero like it does the other digits. Like one, two, three.

As you're doing this reporting, do you find that these conclusions are complementary? Do you think that they're fighting each other? What do you think? Yeah, so I actually... It was funny because I had both groups kind of look at each other's results and I was like, what's going on here? It's slightly different. And they both said that they think, you know, their results are complementary, actually. And the reason for the discrepancy was most likely.

just scale. And, you know, they're hopeful that future experiments will be able to kind of tease apart, you know, what the nuances of this are. So, like, what other, like, research Did they did the researchers talk about like that they would like to do? Yeah, I think that I mean, there's a lot of directions you can go from here. Like it's how the brain comprehends zero. This was kind of the first step into that.

And now, you know, one of the groups actually, they're hoping to go more in the direction of understanding how the brain comprehends absence. Because they think that if they can kind of compare how the brain is processing zero and processing absence, they might be able to see how, like evolutionarily, how the steps toward understanding zero happen. the other group, they're more interested in the numbers aspect of things. So they are hoping to look more into some of these maybe stranger numbers.

For example, like no one looked at the written word zero, which would be a really interesting thing to look at to like, would that look different in the brain than the digit zero or than it would for empty sex? What did you take away from this reporting? You're working on this. You're learning about Xero and the history. What was your big takeaway when you're talking to all of these researchers?

I think my big takeaway is how incredible and complicated and big the brain is. I think my mind is still blown that we have... neurons that are attuned to specific numbers and ways to comprehend these abstract you know, ideas. Because numbers, and zero especially, is an abstraction. And we somehow figured out a way to comprehend it, which is incredible to me. Yeah.

Yasmin, thank you so much for bringing us the story on Xero. Of course. Thank you for being interested. It was super fun to report, so I'm glad others find it cool too. If you want to hear more about how numbers can be tricky for us to comprehend that episode is called how big numbers break our brains. We'll link to it in our show notes. This episode was produced by Hannah Chin and edited by our showrunner, Rebecca Ramirez. Tyler Jones checked the facts. Jimmy Keeley was the audio engineer.

Beth Donovan is our senior director and Colin Campbell is our senior vice president of podcasting strategy. I'm Regina Barber. Thank you for listening to Shortwave from NPR. Support for NPR comes from NPR member stations and Eric and Wendy Schmidt through the Schmidt Family Foundation, working toward a healthy, resilient, secure world for all on the web at theschmidt.org.

This message comes from Wondery. Tis the Grinch Holiday Podcast is back. Listen as the Grinch's celebrity guests try to persuade him that there's more to love about the holiday season. Follow Tiz the Grinch Holiday Podcast on the Wondery app or wherever you get your podcasts. On the embedded podcast from NPR, what is it like to live under years of state surveillance? So many people have fear of losing their families.

For years, the Chinese government has been detaining hundreds of thousands of ethnic Uyghurs. This is the story of one family, torn apart. Listen to The Black Gate on the Embedded podcast from NPR. All episodes are available now.

This transcript was generated by Metacast using AI and may contain inaccuracies. Learn more about transcripts.
For the best experience, listen in Metacast app for iOS or Android
Open in Metacast