Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity - podcast episode cover

Dr. Zachary Knight: The Science of Hunger & Medications to Combat Obesity

Jun 17, 20242 hr 19 minEp. 181
--:--
--:--
Listen in podcast apps:

Episode description

In this episode, my guest is Dr. Zachary Knight, Ph.D., a professor of physiology at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF), and Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI) investigator. We discuss how the brain controls our sense of hunger, satiety, and thirst. He explains how dopamine levels impact our cravings and eating behavior (amount, food choices, etc) and how we develop and can change our food preferences and adjust how much we need to eat to feel satisfied. We discuss factors that have led to the recent rise in obesity, such as interactions between our genes and the environment and the role of processed foods and food combinations. We also discuss the new class of medications developed for the treatment of obesity and diabetes, including the GLP-1 agonists semaglutide (Ozempic, Wegovy) and tirzepatide (Mounjaro). We discuss how these medications work to promote weight loss, the source of their side effects, and the newer compounds soon to overcome some of those side effects, such as muscle loss. Dr. Knight provides an exceptionally clear explanation for our sense of hunger, thirst, and food cravings that translates to practical knowledge to help listeners better understand their relationship to food, food choices, and meal size to improve their diet and overall health. For show notes, including referenced articles and additional resources, please visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman  BetterHelp: https://betterhelp.com/huberman Eight Sleep: https://eightsleep.com/huberman Waking Up: https://wakingup.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Timestamps 00:00:00 Dr. Zachary Knight 00:02:38 Sponsors: BetterHelp, Helix Sleep & Waking Up 00:07:07 Hunger & Timescales 00:11:28 Body Fat, Leptin, Hunger 00:17:51 Leptin Resistance & Obesity 00:20:52 Hunger, Food Foraging & Feeding Behaviors, AgRP Neurons 00:30:26 Sponsor: AG1 00:32:15 Body Weight & Obesity, Genes & POMC Neurons 00:39:54 Obesity, Genetics & Environmental Factors 00:46:05 Whole Foods, Ultra-Processed Foods & Palatability 00:49:32 Increasing Whole Food Consumption, Sensory Specific Satiety & Learning 00:58:55 Calories vs. Macronutrients, Protein & Salt 01:02:23 Sponsor: LMNT 01:03:58 Challenges of Weight Loss: Hunger & Energy Expenditure 01:09:50 GLP-1 Drug Development, Semaglutide, Ozempic, Wegovy 01:19:03 GLP-1 Drugs: Muscle Loss, Appetite Reduction, Nausea 01:23:24 Pharmacologic & Physiologic Effects; GLP-1 Drugs, Additional Positive Effects 01:30:14 GLP-1-Plus Development, Tirzepatide, Mounjaro, AMG 133 01:34:49 Alpha-MSH & Pharmacology 01:40:41 Dopamine, Eating & Context 01:46:01 Dopamine & Learning, Water Content & Food 01:53:23 Salt, Water & Thirst 02:03:27 Hunger vs. Thirst 02:05:46 Dieting, Nutrition & Mindset 02:09:39 Tools: Improving Diet & Limiting Food Intake 02:14:15 Anti-Obesity Drug Development 02:17:03 Zero-Cost Support, Spotify & Apple Follow & Reviews, YouTube Feedback, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter Disclaimer

Transcript

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. My guest today is Dr. Zachary Knight. Dr. Zachary Knight is a professor of physiology at the University of California, San Francisco,

and an investigator with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. For those of you that don't know, Howard Hughes Medical Investigators are selected from an extremely competitive pool of applicants, and have to renew in order to maintain their investigation with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute every five years or so, placing him in the most elite of categories with respect to research scientists.

His laboratory focuses on homeostasis, in particular, what drives our sense of hunger, what drives our sense of thirst, and what controls thermoregulation, which is the ability to maintain body temperature within a specific safe range.

Today we mainly focus on hunger. Dr. Zachary Knight explains the biological mechanisms for craving food, for consuming food, and believe it or not, you have brain circuits that actually determine how much you're likely to eat even before you take your very first bite.

And he explains the biological mechanisms for satiety, that is, the sense that one has had enough of a particular food or food group. Dr. Knight also explains the role of dopamine in food craving and consumption, which I think everybody will find very surprising, because it runs counter current to most people's understanding of what dopamine does in the context of eating and other cravings.

Today's discussion also includes a deep dive into GLP1, glucagon-like peptide, and the novel class of drugs such as ozempic and mongaro and other related compounds that are now widespread in use for the reduction in body weight. Dr. Knight explains how GLP1 was first discovered, and how these drugs were developed, how they work, and importantly, why they work, and how that is leading to the next generation of so-called diet drugs or drugs to treat obesity, diabetes, and related syndromes.

We also discussed thirst, and the intimate relationship between water consumption and food consumption, and we also talk about the relationship between sodium intake, water intake, and food intake.

By the end of today's conversation, you will have learned a tremendous amount about the modern understanding of hunger, thirst, and salt intake, as well as this modern class of drugs such as ozempic and related compounds, all from a truly world-class investigator in the subjects of researching hunger, thirst, and thermal regulation.

Before you begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero-cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is BetterHelp. BetterHelp offers professional therapy with a licensed therapist carried out entirely online.

I've been doing weekly therapy for well over 30 years. Initially, I didn't have a choice. It was a condition of being allowed to stay in high school, but quickly I realized that therapy is an extremely important component to our overall health. In fact, I consider doing regular therapy as important as getting regular cardiovascular exercise and resistance training, which, of course, I also do every week. Now, there are essentially three things that great therapy provides.

First of all, it requires that you have a really good rapport with the therapist, somebody that you can trust and talk to about what's really going on in your life. And, of course, an excellent therapist will provide you support in moving towards the things that are going to grow your life in the best ways. And third, and this is the one that people often overlook, an expert therapist, is somebody who can really provide you useful insights that would not otherwise be obvious to you.

With BetterHelp, they make it very easy to find the therapist with whom you can have those three essential and highly effective components. If you'd like to try BetterHelp, you can go to BetterHelp.com slash Huberman to get 10% off your first month. Again, that's BetterHelp.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by Eight Sleep. Eight Sleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling, heating, and sleep tracking capacity.

Now, I've spoken many times before on this podcast about the critical need for us to get adequate amounts of quality sleep each night. One of the best ways to ensure a great night's sleep is to control the temperature of your sleeping environment. And that's because in order to fall and stay deeply asleep, your body temperature actually has to drop by about 1-3 degrees. And in order to wake up, feeling refreshed and energized, your body temperature actually has to increase by about 1-3 degrees.

Eight Sleep makes it incredibly easy to control the temperature of your sleeping environment by allowing you to program the temperature of your mattress cover at the beginning, middle, and end of the night. I've been sleeping on an eight-sleep mattress cover for well over three years now, and it has completely transformed my sleep for the better. Eight Sleep recently launched their newest generation pod cover, the Pod4 Ultra.

The Pod4 Ultra has improved cooling and heating capacity, higher fidelity sleep tracking technology, and it also has snoring detection that remarkably will automatically lift your head a few degrees to improve your airflow and stop your snoring. If you'd like to try an eight-sleep mattress cover, you can go to 8sleep.com slash Huberman to save $350 off their Pod4 Ultra.

Eight Sleep currently ships to the USA, Canada, UK, select countries in the EU, and Australia. Again, that's 8sleep.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by Waking Up. Waking Up is a meditation app that offers hundreds of guided meditation programs, mindfulness trainings, yoga-need your sessions, and more. I started practicing meditation when I was about 15 years old, and it made a profound impact on my life.

And by now there are thousands of quality peer-reviewed studies that emphasize how useful mindfulness meditation can be for improving our focus, managing stress and anxiety, improving our mood, and much more. In recent years, I started using the Waking Up app for my meditations, because I find it to be a terrific resource for allowing me to really be consistent with my meditation practice.

Many people start a meditation practice and experience some benefits, but many people also have challenges keeping up with that practice. What I and so many other people love about the Waking Up app is that it has a lot of different meditations to choose from. And those meditations are of different durations, so it makes it very easy to keep up with your meditation practice.

Both from the perspective of novelty, you never get tired of those meditations, there's always something new to explore, and to learn about yourself, and about the effectiveness of meditation. And you can always fit meditation into your schedule, even if you only have two or three minutes per day in which to meditate.

I also really like doing yoga ninja, or what is sometimes called non-sleep depress, for about 10 or 20 minutes, because it is a great way to restore mental and physical figure, without the tiredness that some people experience when they wake up from a conventional nap. If you'd like to try the Waking Up app, please go to wakingup.com slash huberman, where you can access a free 30-day trial. Again, that's wakingup.com slash huberman to access a free 30-day trial.

And now for my discussion with Dr. Zachary Knight. Dr. Zachary Knight, welcome. Great to be here. Today we're going to talk about hunger, appetite, thirst, other motivated behaviors, the role of dopamine, the vagus nerve. These are terms and topics that a lot of people hear nowadays, and for which there's a ton of interest.

But just to march us in sequentially, could you describe some of what's happening in the brain and or body as we get hungry, decide what to eat, and then decide that we've had enough to eat? I think most people just assume that my stomach's full is what we said. I've had enough, or we self-regulate it for some other reason, you know, chlore, restriction or monitoring in some cases. What's happening in the brain in terms of the circuitries?

And what have you discovered about what that process looks like in terms of its kind of universality across people, and then maybe how it sometimes differs between people? There's a lot in that that I'll try to unpack, and I can remind of some of the nuance. In other words, as a biologist, as a neuroscientist, how do you think about this thing that we call hunger and appetite?

Absolutely. I think at a very high level, a good way to think about the regulation of food and take by the brain is that there's two systems, short-term system and a long-term system, that are primarily localized to different parts of the brain, operate on different timescales, one on the timescale of a meal, so 10, 20 minutes. And the other on the timescale of sort of weeks, to months, to years, and tracks levels of body fat.

And these two systems sort of interact so that these short-term behaviors we do eating are matched to our long-term need for energy. And so, I think one of the initial experiments that really led to this idea is this great experiment by Harvey Grill, about 50 years ago, it's called the deserabra rat.

And so, essentially, what he did was he made a cut in the rat brains, he took these rats in the lab, made a cut so that he separated the brain stem, so the most posterior part of the brain from the entire four brain. Basically, got rid of, you know, 80% of the rat's brain. So, these basically creating these zombie rats, all they have is a brain stem, and asked, you know, what can these rats still do?

And as you might imagine, they can't do a lot of things, right, because they basically have lost most of their brain. But he discovered that one thing they can still do is regulate the size of a meal. And so, and so, very informative experiment. And so, and you have to be careful how we talk about this, because the way this meal works is you have to actually put food into their mouth, and then they'll swallow it as you put food into their mouth.

But eventually, at some point, they'll start spitting it out. And that basically is an indication that in some sense they're becoming sated. And they're, they're just using the brain stem that they have left, they're able to sense those signals from the gut, and drive the termination of a meal. And he did other experiments showing that many of these signals that come from the gut, gastric stretch, hormones that come from your intestine in response to food and take like CCK.

These deserberat rats just have a brain stem. If you inject those or manipulate the gut in those ways, it can in an appropriate way change how much the rat eats. Now, what can't the rat do when it doesn't have a four brain? And the thing it can't do is it can't respond to longer term changes in energy need. Meaning, if you fast the rat for a couple days, this deserberat rat, then start putting food in its mouth. The amount that it eats doesn't change.

So basically, it doesn't eat a larger meal the way you would if you were fasted for several days and then refed, refed. And that experiment, along with other events, has led to the idea that in the brain stem, and then the most posterior part of your brain, there are neural circuits that control sort of a meal, and then the time scale of 10 minutes or 20 minutes deciding when a meal should end.

And in the four brain, primarily in the hypothalamus, there are neural circuits that then track what is my overall level of energy reserves, what is my level of body fat, things that would fluctuate on time scale of say days when you're fasting. And those four brain centers feedback to talk to the brain stem and modulate those brain stem circuits that are controlling the size of a meal to sort of match these two time scales.

So that's at the highest level how I think about the neural circuits or the controls feeding. There's obviously a lot more going on underneath that. Fascinating. You mentioned body fat and that somehow the brain is tracking the amount of body fat. That caught my ear because while it makes total sense, I'd like to know how that happens if we happen to know the mechanism. And the second question is why body fat and not body fat and muscular mass or body fat and overall body weight.

What is being signaled between body fat and the brain that allows the brain to track body fat? And what do you think body fat is the critical signal? It allows it represents an energy reserve, but certainly there are other things about the bodily state that are important. Yeah, well there are certainly other things about the bodily state that are important and there are other things about physiology definitely that are regulated other than body fat.

But body fat is unique because it represents this energy reserve. So the neural circuitry that regulates eating behavior is in some ways very unique because it has this reserve of energy. So we also study thirst in my lab and drinking and you don't have a reserve of water in your body, right? And that's true for basically everything else. But for fat we have this reserve of energy.

And so it's very important that the brain know how much remains and then adjust behavior and coordinates with that so that you know how urgent it is to get the next meal. And so the thought is that the major signal of the level of body fat that we have is leptin. It's this hormone. It was discovered. It was cloned in 1994 actually by my postdoctoral advisor, a scientist named Jeff Friedman at Rockefeller University.

Although its history goes back way before 1994. So the story behind leptin is that there's a facility called Jackson Labs that you I'm sure familiar with in Maine. And since the 1920s has been raising mice and selling them to academics basically who study physiology and behavior. And so they breed thousands of mice. They should have a nonprofit organization that distributes mice to the scientific community.

And at some point in the 1950s, they spontaneously just because they were breeding so many mice, they came across some spontaneous mutations, mutant mice that were extremely fat. Like the fatest mice they had ever seen, these mice just eat constantly. They're just enormous three times the size of a normal mouse. And it's all body fat. So they're just these huge fat mice.

And they came across several different mutant strains that all had the same phenotype in the sense that they were all extremely fat, all extremely hyperfagic. But they could tell even in the 1950s that these mutations were on different chromosomes. They didn't know anything about how to identify the genes at that point. That was just science fiction. But they knew there were chromosomes and they were on different chromosomes.

And so they labeled one obese, one of these mouse strains obese and the other one diabetes. But they're basically the same. And as people wonder for a long time, what's going on in these mice?

Then there was a scientist at Jackson Labs, Doug Coleman, who had the idea, what if we do an experiment where we connect the circulations of these two different strains of obese mice and test the hypothesis that maybe there's a circulating factor, a hormone that is produced by one of these strains in the controls appetite.

And so that was at that point insulin was known, gluteus, and there were some hormones that were known that were involved in metabolism. So it was logical that there could be a hormone that perhaps regulates body fat levels. And what they found which was remarkable when you attach the obese strain to the DB strain, so you basically connect their circulations of hormones are transmitted between the two.

OBE mouse, that strain dramatically loses weight. In fact, within a couple of weeks, it looks like a normal mouse. It just stops eating, it loses almost all of its body fat. And essentially in all aspects becomes a normal mouse. The DB mouse, nothing really happens. It still remains obese and still remains hyperfagic. And based on just that piece of data, Doug Coleman hypothesized that what was going on is these two mutations were mutations in a hormone and a receptor.

The OBE mouse had a mutation in the hormone that comes from fat. So it couldn't produce this hormone that comes from fat. And signals to the brain how much fat you have. And the DB mouse has a mutation in the receptor, so it can't sense the hormone. And that was just an idea. It was a hypothesis. But in the 1980s, as technology advanced, as it became, there's molecular biology had been invented. It became possible to clone genes.

A number of people tried to identify what are the genetic mutations that are occurring in these mice to make them so obese. And Jeff basically cloned Lepton and showed that in fact, Doug was exactly right. The OBE mutation is a mutation in this hormone, Lepton. And later, a millennium pharmaceuticals showed that the DB mutation is in fact a receptor.

And it was an important discovery for a couple of reasons. One, because this OBE gene is just expressed in fat. It's exclusively expressed in adipose tissue. And how much it's expressed is directly proportional to how much body fat you have. So as you gain weight, the expression of this hormone increases in a linear manner. And then it's to create into the blood. So the level of Lepton in your blood is a direct readout of your body fat reserves.

This receptor for Lepton, Lepton receptor, the functional form of it is expressed almost exclusively in the brain. And it's expressed in all of the brain regions that we knew from previous work were important for appetite. So basically, the expression of this receptor gives you a map in the brain of the neurons that control hunger. And so what happens is basically when you lose weight, the levels of Lepton in your blood fall, because basically you've lost adipose tissue,

the absence of that hormone sends a signal to all these neurons that have Lepton receptors in the brain. They're not getting that signal that I'm starving. And it basically that initiates this entire homeostatic response to starvation. So a big part of that is obviously increased hunger, but it's also decreased energy expenditure, decreased body temperature, even decreased fertility, because you don't want to reproduce if you're starving. Less spontaneous movement.

Less spontaneous movement, all of this. And so the thought is, which I think is absolutely correct, is that this hormone leptin is part of this negative feedback loop from the fat to the brain, that basically tells you about your level of body fat reserves and how urgent it is to find the next meal.

Fascinating. As I recall, Amgine pharmaceuticals own the patent for Lepton in hopes that it would become the blockbuster diet drug, the logic being that if you were to take this hormone somehow, or activate this pathway, that the brain would be tricked into thinking that there was more body fat, more energy reserves than there was, and then people would basically be less hungry, eat less, and lose body fat. Yes. What happened with that? Do we know why it did not work?

Yeah, so that's a great question. So there was a lot of excitement when Lepton was cloned, because it was thought basically we've cured obesity. There was an auction for the patent, Amgine, one, I think it was something like $20 million upfront payment plus royalties, which at the time, I mean, it still is a lot of money, but even more money. Nowadays it would be a drop in the ocean compared to what companies will invest into a potential diet.

Exactly. But you know, at the time, and still a lot of money today. And they did a clinical trial, gave it these people Lepton subcutaneous injections of this hormone, and they didn't lose a lot of weight. And the question was why. And so what was subsequently revealed is that the challenge with Lepton is that individuals who are obese do not have low levels of Lepton for the most part. They actually have high levels of Lepton. And so what they have is the state of Lepton resistance.

So it's analogous to someone who has type 2 diabetes. It's not because they lack insulin. It's because they actually have over time a high level of insulin. And so target tissue stop responding to insulin. And the thought is that it's the same way in obesity and Lepton. Now subsequently, they went back and did an reanalysis of that clinical trial. And as what if you take all of these people and stratify them according to their starting Lepton level.

So some people have relatively low levels of Lepton. Some have higher. Some have really high levels of Lepton. And then ask if we reanalyze the data. How effective is Lepton. And as you might expect, the people with the lowest levels of Lepton, they lost the most weight when you gave them the drug. And the people with the highest levels of Lepton lost the least weight. So there is a rationale there for why for a scenario in which Lepton could work.

Either among the subset of people who just have for some reason lower levels of Lepton. These aren't people with mutations like the OB-MALS. They have some Lepton. They just don't have unusually high levels. Or alternatively after weight loss. So after you've lost a lot of weight, your Lepton levels plummet. They become very low. And that part of the reason it's a big part of the reason it's so difficult to keep weight off is because those Lepton levels are so low.

And so it's been thought for a long time that that that is a scenario where treating treating people with Lepton. Could be really useful to help them keep the weight off. Why it never made it as a drug for that application I really don't understand. Has something to do I think with the pharmaceutical industry with the economics with a bunch of other issues that aren't necessarily scientific. But I think there's still in the future is a possibility that it could come back for that indication.

Especially now that we have these GLP1 drugs and now there's just millions of people losing so much weight. And perhaps they want to transition to a different kind of drug to keep the weight off. We are definitely going to talk about GLP1, OZM-PIC and some of the related compounds in it in a few minutes. But before we do that, I'd love to get to this issue of what's happening in the brain as we get hungry, approach a meal, decide what to eat and decide when we've had enough.

Are there separate circuitries or at least separate neurons for each of those steps? And if you would, could you walk us through what that process looks like since we do it every day, most people do it every day unless they're fasting. Multiple times per day. What's going on in our brain and body as we think about an approach of meal, assume a meal, decide enough. Sure. So there are different neurons that are preferentially involved in different aspects of those processes.

So I think people often divide feeding behavior and many other kinds of motivated behaviors into a petitive and consummatory phase. So a petitive is the phase of the behavior where you're, for example, searching for food. It's foraging. It's all the actions that lead up to the actual behavior itself, which then we call the consummatory phase. That's actually putting the food in your mouth and eating it.

And the general thought is that these four brain circuits in the hypothalamus are more important, particularly in hypothalamus, but other parts of the four brain as well, are more important for the petitive phase. And the brain stem circuits are more important for the consummatory phase. The actual putting it in your mouth and looking chewing, swallowing and all that. Within the hypothalamus, there's a population of neurons called AGRP neurons.

So there's an acronym, AGRP, and stands for a goody-related peptide, but it doesn't really matter. They're absolutely critical for that a petitive phase, for the searching for food, for the desire to find food and consume it when you're hungry. Sorry, just to touch on the AGRP neurons and this a petitive phase, are they known to connect to areas of the brain and body that stimulate the desire to move?

Because I think about when I get hungry, if I'm at my desk or something, I need to get up and find food, me to walk to lunch or go to the refrigerator. Are they somehow linked to the circuits that promote locomotion? Well, they have to promote those things, but they're not directly linked to any of those circuits. They're linked directly to other four brain circuits involved in motivation.

So the way we think about what these kinds of neurons like AGRP neurons are doing, they're not directly talking to the motor circuits to tell you to move your legs or arms to pick up the sandwich or whatever. They're rather creating this general problem that the animal has to solve, which is that I'm hungry, I need to get food. It would be really great if I could have a sandwich. And then the animal uses all of its mental capacities to solve that problem.

So they're just there to set the goal, not so much to direct the solution. And so, but these AGRP neurons, they're a few thousand neurons at the base of the hypothalamus. So basically the most ventral, the most bottom part of the four brain. So tiny population of cells, but outsized importance for the control of feeding behavior. So if you stimulate these cells in a mouse or a rat that's not hungry, the animal will voraciously eat like it's starving.

If you silence these cells, animals will starve to death. So you can basically give them food, they just won't need to voluntarily until basically you have to euthanize them because they're they've lost so much weight. And the activity of these these AGRP neurons, it's thought to track the bodies need for energy. One reason that's thought is that they express these receptors for leptin.

This hormone that was just talking about that that comes from fat and signals the level of body fat reserves. And leptin inhibits AGRP neurons. So as you might expect, if you have lots of body fat, then a neuron that expresses that controls hunger should be less active than if you have very little body fat. So that's one mechanism by which leptin controls hunger.

We, my lab, have investigated the role of these AGRP neurons from a slightly different perspective, which is and this relates to your question about what happens when we approach food, when we start a meal. And to ask, what are their activity patterns? What is the natural sort of firing of this population of neurons? One animal eats a meal. It's a very basic question.

Something I think we've wanted to know for a long time. Was not really addressable until about 10 years ago because it's the technology didn't exist because these are such a tiny population of cells so deep in the brain. So one of the very first experiments we did in my lab was to investigate that to ask for the first time what happens to these AGRP neurons when an animal eats.

And so, my first graduate student's human chin, he used a technology called fiber photometry, which allows us to put a fiber optic into the mouse's brain so that the moudame could record fluorescence from these AGRP neurons, which we could use as a readout of their activity. It's basically using a calcium sensor, so calcium is a surrogate for neural activity.

And one of the very first experiments he did was to make the animal hungry. These AGRP neurons will be very active because the animal is hungry, and then let's give it some food and see what happens during a meal. And our expectation was that these AGRP neurons would gradually decline in activity as the animal eats and levels of hormones and the blood start changing, feeding back to inhibit these neurons.

What we found was really surprising. I remember that when he made this discovery, basically running into my office and saying, I gave the mouse a piece of food, but the weirdest thing happened, the neurons shut off almost immediately. And I said, you may be in a mistake, it's okay, you're just starting off in graduate school, this happens, go back and repeat the experiments, and then we'll discuss it.

But he did several times, he said, every single time I do this happens, I give a hungry mouse food and the AGRP neurons within just a few seconds, their activity has greatly diminished back to the level that would be in a fed mouse, even before they take the first bite of food.

He then went to do a series of experiments, try to understand what was going on, and what he basically showed by changing the kind of food he gave them or the accessibility of the food or how hungry the mouse was in measuring the response of these AGRP neurons was that what the neurons were doing was predicting the mouse looks at the food, looks at how palatable it is, imagines how hungry the mouse is, how accessible it is.

And then within a few seconds these neurons predict how much food the mouse is going to eat in the forthcoming meal. So essentially these neurons know how much the mouse is going to eat before the mouse even takes the first bite.

And you can show this by very simple analysis in which you give the mouse different foods and you look at how much these AGRP neurons drop when the mouse sees and smells the food. And then you plot that, again, this drop happens in three seconds, four seconds, something like that. And you look at how much does the mouse go on to eat in the next 30 minutes, you can just draw straight line. This was one of the first results from my lap, and it was really surprising to all of us.

I think everyone, but it illustrated a theme that we've now seen again and again, which is that these circuits the control internal state, things like hunger and thirst. What they're constantly doing is predicting the future.

And you can sense these signals from the body that tell you about what's happened, but those signals are slow. And you don't want to wait 20 minutes from the food that you ingested to reach your stomach and then slowly start entering your intestine to figure out what was the nutrient content of the meal. You want to try to figure that out as soon as you can.

And so the animals learn, presumably through just experience that okay, something that smells like this and looks like this that has about this many calories, and I'm known this hungry, so I'm going to eat about this much. And that information is all transmitted to these circuits to start the process of satiation before the meal begins.

Is it satiation or it's ceasing of foraging so that the animal or if I translate to a person decides, okay, now I'm going to consume this sandwich, this package of food. That's a great question. So we don't fully know the answer.

So the one interpretation of the data I just showed you is what you exactly what you said is that what these neurons do is they control foraging alone, they don't control eating. And so this is perfect. You see the food, you know, it's these, it's got enough calories, the neurons shut off and then you stay there and eat it. You transition from this repetitive to this consummatory phase.

But that doesn't seem to be the whole explanation because if you artificially stimulate these neurons, so prevent that drop from ever happening, just stimulate them continually. You also just sit there and eat. So you can't fully separate, although we like to make the distinction between a repetitive and consummatory and we know that that in different parts of the brain, that's more important for one versus the other.

The reality is that the entire behavior is linked and you can't fully separate them. So there's a number of ideas about what this means. So one idea that I just mentioned is that starting the process of satiety before the meal begins. Another idea, which you mentioned, which could be part of the answer is that it is reducing the repetitive drive and allowing the transition to consummatory behavior.

Another idea is that, and I call these ideas because we don't really fully know the answer yet for exactly what the purpose is. And biology is always hard answer why something happens. You can figure out what does happens, but then you can, the reason why it evolved that way is challenging.

Another idea is it's involved in these what we call cephalic phase responses that are necessary to prepare you for a meal. So the famous example, this is Pavlov, basically, trains the dog to associate the ring of the bell with the presentation of food and then eventually the ring of the bell alone causes the dog to salivate in the absence of any food.

And salivation is one example of a cephalic phase response. The purpose of that is to have enzymes in your mouth that basically are going to digest the food and get them there right before you need them. But there's also some other things like basically this creation of insulin occurs in response to food cues, changes in gastric acid, got motility. All these things are getting ready for the for the meal to happen.

And so another idea is it could be part of that, but it probably is doing all of these things. As many of you know, I've been taking AG1 for more than 10 years now. So I'm delighted that they're sponsoring this podcast to be clear. I don't take AG1 because they're a sponsor rather. They are a sponsor because I take AG1.

In fact, I take AG1 once and often twice every single day and I've done that since starting way back in 2012. There is so much conflicting information out there nowadays about what proper nutrition is. But here's what there seems to be a general consensus on. Whether you're an omnivore, a carnivore, a vegetarian or a vegan, I think it's generally agreed that you should get most of your food from unprocessed or minimally processed sources, which allows you to eat enough, but not overeat,

get plenty of vitamins and minerals, probiotics and micronutrients that we all need for physical and mental health. Now I personally am an omnivore and I strive to get most of my food from unprocessed or minimally processed sources. But the reason I still take AG1 once and often twice every day is that it ensures I get all of those vitamins, minerals, probiotics, etc.

But it also has adaptogens to help me cope with stress. It's basically a nutritional insurance policy meant to augment, not replace, quality food. So by drinking a serving of AG1 in the morning and again in the afternoon or evening, I cover all of my foundational nutritional needs. And I like so many other people that take AG1, report feeling much better in a number of important ways such as energy levels, digestion, sleep and more.

So while many supplements out there are really directed towards obtaining one specific outcome, AG1 is foundational nutrition designed to support all aspects of well-being related to mental health and physical health. If you'd like to try AG1, you can go to drinkag1.com slash huberman to claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs with your order plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's drinkag1.com slash huberman.

It's so interesting. I have a number of questions, but I think the one that I'll put at the top of the list is the other night we were out to dinner in New York. And I was very hungry. I hadn't eaten much that day. And I was looking forward to a nice steak. They brought out bread. French bread was for interest. I took one bite. I realized it was absolutely delicious. French bread. The butter was fantastic. And so I had some bread and butter, which I love.

Then they brought more. And then they started bringing out, I don't know who ordered them because I didn't appetizers. And I realized that this was going to be a much more extensive, chlorically dense meal. And suddenly my appetite for the appetizers is sort of went down because I knew there was more food coming.

Had I not known that there was more food coming, I think I would have consumed more of the appetizers, which also looked great. So clearly there's something going on with these agrp neurons.

Based on new information, exactly. On the other end of the spectrum, I did a solo episode about eating disorders in anorexia nervosa in particular. And one of the things that I learned from experts in that field, the psychiatrists who work on this and the scientists who work on this, is that people with anorexia are unbelievably tuned to the caloric content of food.

That their visual system and presumably other systems have become like almost hyper accurate calculators of the amount of calories in food. They've devoted a lot of cognition to it. It sometimes can, you know, border on or be placed within the obsessive realm. But that they see food and they they can tell you a tremendous amount about the caloric amounts with these foods, even food combinations to, you know, with a very small margin of error.

And that drives in that condition, obviously, a food avoidance. So I have to assume that these agrp neurons are involved in this kind of thing. One represents a regulation, in the case of the example I gave in the other case, a, let's just call it what it is because anorexia nervosa is the most deadly of the psychiatric condition, sadly, a pathologic dysregulation, a maladaptive dysregulation.

So what is known about these agrp neurons in humans, meaning do they exist in humans? Presumably they express the leptin receptor. It sounds like they are able to integrate information, both cognitive based on immediate experience, visual, olfactory, but also a lot of prior experience. You know, a hamburger patty, I don't can't tell you how many calories it has. All I know is that it's mostly protein and some fat.

You know, what are these neurons doing? What do they have access to? They sound like, you know, when any time I hear about hypothalamus, I think very basic dries, but you're talking about a pretty sophisticated analysis of a real time event that is driving fairly nuanced behavioral decisions and updating that, which is a big deal.

You know, we're both neuroscientists, but for everyone listening and watching, this is a big deal. This is as nuance is deciding whether or not somebody is friend or foe or deciding whether or not you, you like a movie or you don't. I mean, this is some, some pretty sophisticated processing. This isn't eat, don't eat or eat less. These are switches. These are dials.

Exactly. Yeah, so there's a lot there. I'll try to unpack that. So the first thing I would say is they are present in humans and humans do humans have age, your penurons, human age, your penurons express the leptin receptor. And we think the functions are very similar. So one of the nice things actually about studying these kinds of things, like basic mechanisms of hunger, thirst, because these things are so important for survival, they've been under really strong selection.

And so many of the components of these systems are genetically hardwired, meaning these are cell types that have a single purpose in this case to control hunger. They're labeled by specific genes and those are conserved through revolution. We also know that this pathway, this age, your penuron pathway, is important in humans due to human genetics.

So just add a little bit more information here. There's a companion set of neurons called palm cenerons that promote satiety. So the sort of the Yin and Yang of hunger, age, your penurons promote hunger, palm cenerons promote satiety. They're intermingled in the same part of the hypothalamus. They're axons that project to the exact same downstream brain regions.

And it's thought that these two neurons compete with each other to control appetite. And that competition occurs through neuropeptides that they release, one of which is an agonist for a downstream receptor and the other one of which is an antagonist. We know from human genetics that these that among severely obese people, mutations in this pathway, age, your pen, palm cenerons and their direct downstream targets are quite common.

So anyway, to say that some amount of obesity is genetic in nature at the level of neuronal firing or circuitry. I think a lot of bodyweight regulation is genetic. It's highly heritable. There's a question of how much of it is due to single genes. And the number of people quote, and this is among people who are severely obese.

So not just people who you've seen someone who's overweight, but people have sort of syndromes where they're very obese from a very young age. Among those people, something on the order of 10% have mutations in this pathway. And it can either be this hormone palm c, or an enzyme within those cells that processes palm c into the right form, or in the down, and this is the most common mutation in the downstream receptor for palm c.

It's called the melanocortin-4 receptor. And so among the severely obese people have sort of genetically inherited severe obesity from childhood. Something on the order of 10% have mutations in this pathway. So it's very clear that this pathway is involved in bodyweight regulation in humans. Most obesity, although there's a very strong genetic component, is not associated with single gene mutations, like this, such as effects of many mutations.

But we know that even in that sort of polygenic obesity that has many different genetic causes, that the brain is important. And one of the reasons we know that is if you look at the genes through genetic association studies that have been associated with bodyweight, and there's been lots of genetic association studies trying to find mutations that are associated with whether you're lean or obese.

Something on the order of 1,000 genes have been linked to bodyweight regulation. And the vast majority of those are expressed in the brain. They're highly enriched for brain processes, which makes sense because bodyweight is controlled by food and take, right? And the brain controls behavior, and also the brain controls energy expenditure.

So maybe it's not so surprising, but it's clear that mutations in genes in the brain are important for bodyweight, which is consistent with the results of twin studies. So if you look at monosegotic versus diosegotic twins, the estimates for the heritability of bodyweight is something on the order of 80%. We should explain monosegotic, diosegotic, which I've talked about before in the podcast, just to show people that.

Just identical versus fraternal twins, basically. And so, and by comparing their, basically, their, their bodyweight when they become adults, you can get a sense for how much of this is genetic versus environmental. And something on the order of 80% is thought of the variation between individuals is thought to be have a genetic component. So I don't think most people appreciate that.

Yeah, and a lot of the debate we hear nowadays is because there are things that people can do to lose body fat, exercise, eat differently, etc. Maybe embrace pharmacology, if that's appropriate. There seems to be this, to me, silly debate as to whether or not people should be eating better and exercising or assuming that all of the obesity they might have arises through genetic causes. And therefore take a prescription drug. I mean, why wouldn't it be a combination of things?

Yeah, like to me, it just seems like why wouldn't people embrace some or all of the tools that they could afford and that are safe for them. So I just want to get that out there because the moment this comes up, people start thinking, oh, well, the moment we assign a genetic source to something, we're removing personal responsibility.

But of course, there are people. I know people who have struggled with their weight their entire lives for whom some of these new pharmaceuticals like ozempic have provided them the opportunity to finally be able to lose weight and feel better and exercise safely, for instance. I completely agree with that. I think there is a misconception out there about this about what it means for something to be genetically heritable.

And I think this gets to the root of why so many people find this sort of hard to believe that there's such a strong genetic component to body weight. And that's the idea that, you know, if you look at people say 75 years ago, right, they were much leaner, right, and you look at people today and there's been this starting some time around, you know, the 1970s, there's this explosion in body weight and increase in obesity.

Is that when that's when it started mid-September, sort of 1970s is a matter of a lot of start to snacking. So there's lots of explainations. So by the way, I don't think that's the reason folks, I think there are a lot of reasons, but the theories that that abound right now on social media, or I have a list of the theories as to why the obesity is increasing and everything from seed oils to snacking to smartphones to conspiracies to its wild.

Yeah, yeah, it's wild. The range of hypotheses is wild. I mean, the challenges, I mean, some of them could be true, but it's just very hard to test those things, so sure, immensely, because they're happening in the whole population, right.

But so I think the thing that people find hard to wrap their heads around because it is a little bit of a confusing idea is that how can it be that in say 50 or 75 years, there's been this explosion in obesity, which is the environment has changed, but human genetics has not changed in that amount of time. Sometimes it's not fast enough for people to evolve, so can't be due to mutations in humans. What about devolve? My understanding is that within a species, evolving new traits is very slow.

Yes. But mutations arise like the OB mutation, and then you can get very fat versions of an animal very quickly, right. All you need is a, sure. If it's a recessive allele, you need two copies, and the next thing you know, you've got a mouse that's four times larger than a typical mouse, and it's all explained by increased body weight. So that can happen very quickly within a species.

What's rare to find is an entire new branch of a species that has a very new adaptive function. That seems more rare. So that's true. So definitely there's some things that take longer to evolve than others, but with humans we're talking about just two generations. There just isn't enough time for any evolution of any security. We would be me, baby boomers, right, generations. And then whatever is wizzy millennial, that's true.

Exactly. Exactly. So I think the thing that people find hard to wrap their heads around is how can it be that this is how, that increase in body weight is clearly environmental, right. Because that's all this change is the environment. It's nothing has changed genetically. Yet it's also true what I said that body weight is extremely hierarchical.

It's one of the most heritable features and something on the order of 80% the only thing that one of the only things we know about that's actually more heritable than body weight is height, right. Most diseases are not as heritable as body weight. How can you explain that? The idea is this. There's a distribution of body weight among people. So in any given society, at any point in time, some people are going to be leaner, some people are going to be more obese.

That distribution, where you lie on that distribution is determined primarily by genetics. So you may be the person who has the thrifty genes, so that basically cause you to save energy. And so you would be more on the obese side, or you may be maybe a person who has different genes that cause you to be a little bit less hungry, so you would be on the leaner side.

What environment does is then it shifts that whole distribution, so that basically the mean shifts so that everyone becomes or most people become heavier. And so sort of a phrase that people sometimes use is that genetics loads the gun and environment pulls the trigger. So basically genetics sets your propensity and then environment can basically unmask that.

And so as we've had this change in environment where there's all of this, and we don't know exactly what the things are that have changed, they're important, but there's all this ultra-process food, highly palatable food, various other things that you mentioned, seed oils, who knows what that's important.

Certain people had these latent mutations that made them say very sensitive to palatable food, and in an earlier time they may have been lean, but now because they have that latent capacity to be sensitive to ultra-processed food, they now gain tons of weight in the environment that we're in. It's still because of genetics, but it also requires the environmental component.

I mean, you just take a step back, right? You can make anyone lean, but just putting them, putting them in prison and just only feeding them 1500 calories. I mean, we've done those kinds of experiments. This is a famous experiment, the Minnesota starvation experiment, right?

They basically, they put people in prison, but this is in World War II, they took a bunch of healthy volunteers, fed them 1600 calories a day, and just asked what would happen if you basically send me staffed people, and unsurprisingly they lose an incredible amount of weight, all they think about is food, they basically their body temperature goes down, their heart rate goes down, they just become obsessed with food.

And you could always do that for anyone, right? But in a given environment where you're not in that kind of situation, then your propensity to gain weight will be determined by genetics. So that's the idea. I very much appreciate that description, and I know a great number of other people will as well, because the explanation for the increase in obesity has not been described with that level of accuracy and detail with respect to the interactions between genetics and the environment.

Is it fair to say that what's changed in our environment is the free availability of food? I was walking through an airport yesterday and every 20 meters or so, there's a vending machine or a restaurant. The cost of calories is fairly low, right? Getting high quality nutritious food that tastes great is expensive. I would argue. But getting calories is fairly inexpensive.

I think that's a plausible hypothesis. It's one of several plausible hypotheses, and it would be surprising to me if it didn't contribute. But the reality of these population level questions is just so hard to actually know, because you can't do an experiment, right? We can't create a parallel society where we manipulate one of these variables and see if the people become obese. So I think probably the availability of food, the free availability, the low cost is one part of it.

Another part of it is probably, although it's not proven, is that these ultra-processed foods have a number of features that make people prone to gain weight. And this is really beautiful work. If you know what this is from Kevin Hall at the NIH who's investigated this, he's really my opinion, the best person doing this kind of human obesity research today.

He does these experiments where he takes people into the NIH and to the hospital, hospitalizes them for several weeks, so he can exactly control what they eat. And he did this beautiful experiment, where basically he had chefs prepare two kinds of food, one ultra-processed,

and the other, not ultra-processed, sort of, more whole foods, more healthier foods. But had them take a lot of care so that when they gave the foods to independent radars, to people to test, they would say, this is about equally palatable. This ultra-processed dish as much as this non-ultra-processed dish. What's an example of an ultra-processed dish, like an out-of-package macaroni and cheese. Exactly, with bacon kind of thing. Exactly.

Versus some pasta sitting next to a vegetable and some nice piece of salmon or something. Exactly, exactly. And took people into the hospital, basically allowed them to eat just as much as they would like, first of the ultra-processed meals. And then they had the selection of ultra-processed meals for a couple weeks, and then switched them to the non-ultra-processed meals.

And then also did it in the reverse order, so they had the people that got the regular food first, then they got the ultra-processed food. And what he found is that even though people rated the foods as equally palatable, they ate much more of the ultra-processed food. And they actually gained weight during that two-week period when they were being given the ultra-processed foods. And then when you switched them, they lost weight.

So the idea being that you can have two sets of foods that you have equal preferences for, but something about the ultra-processed food is making you eat more of it when you actually consume it. And there's a number of ideas about why that could be. So one idea is that these ultra-processed foods have been optimized, have the right percentage of fat and sugar and protein to sort of promote more consumption once you start eating it, so that could be part of it.

Another idea is that a big thing about whole foods is that they take more energy to digest and they have more volume. So one of the striking things from that study is if you just look at the pictures of the meals, they're the same number of calories, but there's so much more food seemingly on the non-processed food versus the ultra-processed food. And that's just because whole foods are bigger because they're not so energy dense.

So, and we know that, for example, volume is a major signal on the short term for regulating food and takes. So, we just eat more volume that could be valuable. And there's lots of things like that. So, I think that's another plausible hypothesis, but the truth is we don't really know.

I have a hypothesis and I don't want to force you into speculation, but given that you've studied and discovered that the neurons and circuits involved in a petitive and consummatory behaviors can learn based on experience and expectation,

and I think it's fair to game to at least ask your thoughts on this. So, I've been paying a lot of attention to the landscape of what the general public think about, let's call them elimination diets, where people will just eat meat, or go into a vegan diet, or do some time restricted feeding, or do any number of different things that have been shown to promote weight loss, provided people obey the laws of thermodynamics and consume fewer calories than they... Yeah.

...then they burn. I do believe in calories and calories out. And there are a number of different routes to get there, and some are more painful, some are less painful, and it depends on the individual lifestyle, exercise, and on and on. But let's just suppose for a moment, based on Kevin's work on highly processed foods, versus whole foods, that there's a learning that takes place when we eat.

And that this learning takes place over time, such that our brain and appetite start to link the variables of taste, macro nutrients, proteins, fats, and carbohydrates, sort of knowledge about macro nutrients, a piece of fish is mostly protein, has some fat, a bowl of rice is mostly carbohydrate, has some protein.

Put a pad of butter on it, has some fat also, right? It is sort of obvious. But taste, macro nutrient content, calories, which we already know, people with anorexia are exquisitely good at counting with their eyes.

So it's possible, they represent again a pathologic extreme of this. And micro nutrient content, maybe even amino acid content, like how much lucine is there. Now most people aren't thinking about how much lucine is in a meal, but we know that lucine is important for certain aspects of muscle metabolism, it's present in certain proteins and not others, you're going to find less of it in a vegetable, typically than you would in a piece of chicken, and so on.

And that when people eat mostly non-processed or minimally processed foods, and not in combination, so we're not talking about stewing all this together or blending all of it together, which sounds disgusting, right? Broccoli rice and a chicken breast blend together, just sounds horrible, eating them separately, if there's some olive oil and a little pad of butter involved, like that sounds pretty good.

But a highly processed food in some ways is a blending together of macronutrients, micronutrients, if there are any, and other features of the food that neurons in the brain seem to pay attention to, and then giving it a unified taste, a Dorito, right?

A candy bar that we attach to the product, we attach to the name of the processed food to the packaging, but I could imagine, and here's the hypothesis, that is quote-unquote confusing to our neural circuits in a way that doesn't match up well with our thermodynamic requirements of how much we're burning versus how much we need to eat.

Whereas when I eat a piece of steak and a vegetable, I actually want less carbohydrate afterwards. If I eat the carbohydrate first, it's difficult because I love the taste of carbohydrates, especially when they're combined with fat, but there seems to be an easier time regulating food intake when people step back and say, I'm going to consume minimally processed whole foods, and I'm guessing it's not just because they're trying to be healthier, that might be what stimulates the shift,

but that the brain starts to learn the relationship between food volume, smell, taste, what these things look like, and satiation at the level of, oh, that's enough amino acids because I had a piece of fish. So maybe I don't need to consume as much of some other things, or the vegetables provide volume and fiber, and often vegetables can taste really delicious too, so that there's a, there's a linking of nutrients, calories and taste in a way that's more appropriately matched to the energetic demands of the organism.

Yes, yes. In this case, us humans that highly processed foods bypass. Yeah. Okay, now I realize that was long winded and forgive me, but my audience is used to that. Whenever I'm trying to table something for no pun intended, for a discussion that I would like to think and at least stimulate some additional thinking about a landscape, in this case, nutrition and feeding behavior that for a lot of it was just really confusing.

And this is the last thing I'll say I have several friends who have been very overweight their entire lives for whom the following diet has worked exceptionally well. I'm not a diet coach, I'm not nutritionist, I don't pretend to be one. I say eat proteins like meat, fish eggs, vegetables and fruit, and do that for a couple of months and then add back in starches as you see fit based on your food intake.

And without fail, they all lose a ton of weight. They're very happy with that. They add back in a minimum of starches. They keep the weight off and they're also exercising, but not more than they were before in most cases. And I don't think that it's meat or fish or vegetables per se. I think it's that they finally develop an appreciation for what different foods have in terms of what they actually need.

And without fail, they all say, oh, you know, I went to this party and I had a piece of cake and it didn't taste good to me after three or four bites. So that's interesting too. So I just would like your thoughts on this. We're not defining any new diets, I don't sell any diets, I don't do any of that.

But I find it amazing that when people start eating minimally processed whole foods, I have to assume that their brain changes as it relates to appetite craving and just kind of an unconscious understanding about what food is providing them or not. So highly processed foods basically bypass all of this and just get you to consume more. Perhaps in hopes of getting something that you probably aren't getting at all or that you need to consume a lot of this food in order to get.

There's several interesting ideas there. There's two that come to mind just thinking about what you just said. So the one is the idea of what's going on when these people consume simpler diets, more of a whole foods and one thing I think that's very likely going on is this phenomenon of sensory specific satiety is being engaged.

And so sensory specific satiety is just the idea that as you expose yourself repeatedly to a certain flavor or taste, you basically lose appetite for that you get specific loss of appetite for that flavor or taste. So this is why as you said, basically if you start off eating the protein after all, I don't want any more salmon, but I would like some carbohydrates now because you have the sensory specific satiety.

And so it's well known actually that if you simplify your diet, make your diet really simple. So there's just a few things that sensory specific satiety alone can cause you to eat less basically because there's just less variety in your diet. You don't want to eat more of that same thing. And so I think a lot of diets actually is not about the specific macronutrient or the specific food is just that they're reducing the variety in the diet.

Eventually you just get sick of eating the same thing. And you know, this is the thought behind that idea is that it's important evolutionarily so that you eat a diverse diet. It's the reason probably that you want sweets after you've eaten a savory meal and so on.

A second idea though that comes to mind is just as you mentioned this idea of learning and then so much about our preferences for food are they're not an eight they're driven by learning right and so you know there are some things they're an eight so if you put sugar on a baby's top it's not you know smile indicating that it likes it and if you put something bitter it'll frown and a rat will do the same thing and you need to rad.

But most of flavor and the perception of food is not just sweet or bitter is this much more complex sensation that involves smells it involves taste and then it involves how those tastes and smells interact with the post ingest of effects of the nutrients so the sensing of those nutrients in your stomach and in your intestine primarily in your intestine are thought to then feedback and then change your preference for these foods and so you know there's lots of examples of this that you can just bet from every day experience.

Most people the first time they had a beer or the first time they had a glass of coffee found it repulsive right because it's extremely bitter but then we come to crave these things because we know what they do to our body we like what they do to our body and that doesn't just make us take them like their medicine we actually somehow change our very perception of how that flavor is we actually come to savor that flavor we previously found disgusting and it's because our sensation of what is what is what whether something is good or bad depends on an internal state and so it's an interesting idea you know perhaps if these ultra processed foods that are in the

process of so many different ingredients and such an unnatural combination perhaps this process of learning about the nutrient content of different foods and flavors becomes impaired because it's just the brain is not used the brains used to saying you know this is a piece of chicken and this is primarily protein and so I can gauge you know from this

and I can connect this flavor to an amino acid content but something that's so diverse it might be harder to do and isn't it the case that the neurons in the gut and the hormones that are produced by the gut as we digest food and that the neurons in the brain that control appetite and feeding have to be tuned to macronutrient content because those are the primary colors of nutrients and nutrients are the brain that we are using.

So we are the way in which we can persist on a day to day basis right I mean I'm not trying to sound more sophisticated where simpler terms would suffice what I'm busy saying is that the neurons in our brains that control these behaviors both eating and cessation of eating an ingredient or an entire meal can't be tuned to a particular food product or to chicken or to an egg or to a steak or to lentils but rather to amino acid content essential amino acid content.

In particular essential fatty acids and in the case of carbohydrate whatever is going to replace whatever glycogen we might have depleted right I mean like we really break it down into biology eating is for a purpose and my understanding is that the purpose of eating is to replace those things as needed rather than to you know taste savory or taste.

You know absolutely those are just those those things those sensory cues are just markers that tell the brand what might be in that substance I think if you if you look at broadly at this difference between calories and macronutrients and micronutrients I would say what you see is that most of the circuits that are controlling hunger are primarily calories specific so they they can like for example an agar pinaron I can put sugar fat or protein into the stomach of a mouse and to an equal extent inhibit an agar pinar and as long as they have equal

calories really yeah so a little drop of olive oil into the belly that has of an animal that has and let's drop let's say a little bit more let's say 120 calories of olive oil is equal potent to 120 calories of chicken breast at the level of these agar pinarons it is so we don't care about the match no they're really concerned about

energy there are circuitries that are more concerned with macronutrients individually although I don't think we know nearly as much about how that works and I think the evidence is clear that the strongest defended macronutrient by far is protein so protein you know I don't think really sugar and fat intake are strongly defended in the sense that you can you you're fine if you go without eating sugar right basically you can synthesize sugar from other from you know assets for example and you don't develop.

And you don't develop. And a specific sugar appetite in the same way you do for example if you deprive yourself of hungry and you've all protein hunger or essential I think the difference is that proteins consists of essential amino acids there's this I forget if it's nine I think amino acids that you your body cannot synthesize you absolutely need or you will die and so.

Whereas sugar in fact can be interchange with other macronutrients so and there's other things also that you absolutely need to ingest like sodium chloride right so sodium so there's very few deprive an animal of sodium they'll develop the salt appetite that's incredible basically and that's completely innate but that's I think salt appetite and protein appetite or the things that are probably the most strongly regulated at the level of the macro micronutrients.

I'd like to take a brief break and acknowledge one of our sponsors element element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need that means the electrolyte sodium magnesium and potassium in the correct amounts and ratios and nothing you don't which means no sugar now I and others on this podcast have talked about the critical importance of hydration for proper brain and body functioning even a slight degree of dehydration can diminish cognitive and physical performance it's also important that you get adequate electrolytes the electrolytes sodium magnesium and potassium are critical.

For the functioning of all the cells in your body especially your neurons your nerve cells drinking element dissolved in water makes it very easy to ensure that you're getting adequate hydration and adequate electrolytes to make sure I'm getting proper amounts of hydration and electrolytes I dissolve one packet of element in about 16 to 32 ounces of water when I wake up in the morning and I drink that basically first thing in the morning I'll also drink element dissolved in water during any kind of physical exercise I'm doing especially in hot days when I'm sweating a lot losing water and elective.

They have a bunch of different great tasting flavors of element my favorite is the watermelon although I confess I also like the raspberry and the citrus basically I like all the flavors and element has also just released a new line of canned sparkling element so these aren't the packets you dissolve in water these are cans of element that you crack open like any other can drink like a soda but you're getting your hydration and your electrolytes with no sugar if you'd like to try element you can go to drink element spelled LMNT dot com slash Hubertman to claim your first

free element sample pack with the purchase of any element drink mix again that's drink element dot com slash Hubertman to claim a free sample pack if we could talk about body weight homeostasis for a moment that I think that would be useful so let's say somebody decides they want to lose some weight they

are more restricted slightly either by exercising more eating lesser both their body weight drops by a bit let's say they lose 10 pounds eight of which our body fat they lose a little bit of lean mass also they're now at a new lower body weight are the

neurons motivated to have them seek out more food in other words are they hungrier and more motivated to find an eat food or do these a g r p neurons learn hey body weight is lower and I don't need to push to find so much food so often no I mean the idea is that the age of the

p neurons are more active when you lose weight and that that chronic activation of those neurons and part because leptin levels are lower in the blood because you've lost weight is that drive that that counter regulatory drive that drives you to then consume more food but then how do people ever keep weight off well so part of the answer is they don't I mean so so there's so really I would argue like I have these friends who were very heavy most of the excess weight was body body

fat for a long time they seem to be doing great yeah eating the way that I described before by the way I'm not a proponent of any one particular diet I vegan friends carnivore friends but that pattern of eating I described before has been enormously successful for them I haven't run a you know a randomized control trial it's not my job to do that in the realm of nutrition but

they're doing great they claim to be stated they are so happy with the way things are going and I don't hear that they're constantly hungry I hear that they're constantly say that well so I would say that that you know there have been efforts for a long time to develop diets that would help people consistently lose weight and it's been very unsuccessful

there are some people who for various reasons can successfully lose weight and keep it off and I don't know that I have a good answer for what's going on in those individual cases how they are the exceptions to the rule what about them is different that makes sense and I think that's a good way to get a good drink alcohol yeah so there's other things so you know I think so behavioral regulations better when you're so opposed to change your environment but you know so

what this is sort of getting at is what is the counter regulatory response to weight loss and so this has been studied it was first studied in the context of energy expenditure and because energy expenditure is actually surprisingly easier to measure in humans than food intake because people don't tell you accurately what food they if they're free living humans they have to fill out a question

there but and the idea is that for every kilogram of weight you lose so it's about 2.2 pounds I think your energy expenditure decreases by about 30 kilo calories a day now so not a ton but that is significant right 30 calories and then if you lose as you said 10 pounds then that's 150 calories and that adds up over time one interesting thing about that is that if you take people who were obese and then they've lost a ton of weight so there's a study by Rudy Libel about 25 years ago that did this

take people lost like a hundred pounds and then take a control group that has the same height weight basically the same body composition as those people who've now lost a hundred pounds compare their energy expenditure the energy expenditure in the people that lost all the weight is about 25% lower than the people who never were obese and so those people who lost the weight we call them the reduced obese so that's what they were called in those studies

and the idea is that there's this now this chronic deficit they have to eat 25% less than someone who looks the same as them as the same height as them same weight as them in order to maintain that body weight

what's unclear is whether that's because those people simply always had a little slow metabolism they're always destined to be obese and then you just basically you're comparing two different groups or whether something about the process of gaining weight and being in a higher weight for a longer period of time changes the brain so that then once you lose the weight it's irreversible

but there have been studies looking at at least a year and it doesn't seem to come back within a year that difference in energy expenditure now a question is is that really the big effect is that why it's so hard to lose weight energy expenditure or is it because you're hungrier and that's actually much harder to measure but there's another really nice study again by Kevin Hall investigating this used a really clever approach this drug

so basically what he wanted to do was is he reasoned that you can measure people's body weight and you can measure people's energy expenditure and because calories and calories out if we can measure body weight and energy expenditure accurately we can then back calculate how much that person was actually eating

so let's see what happens when you have people lose weight how does their food intake change but the trick to this is you need to do it in such a way that you don't just tell them to go run on a treadmill

because if you tell somebody to go around treadmill in those ways then basically they're thinking about the fact that they're doing this you need to do it in some way covertly so that you increase the energy expenditure cause them to lose weight but without them realizing that's what's happening

so they gave them these drugs these sGLT2 inhibitors and so pill you can take they're used for diabetes they block this protein sGLT2 in the kidney that is necessary for glucose to be reabsorbed into the blood and so basically it happens you pee out about like 90 grams of glucose a day but you don't know that you're doing that and that causes you to lose energy and these people will lose some weight

and then measure how their food intake changes and what that showed is that for every two pounds or so of weight you lose your hunger goes up by 100 calories per day so basically you've got a 30 kilocalorie decrease in energy expenditure 100 kilocalorie decrease in appetite for every two pounds you lose on average some people will be exceptions and they won't experience that at all for aspects of their physiology we don't understand

and so the increased hunger seems to be the main reason people find it so difficult to keep weight off that seems the perfect segue to talk about GLP1, glucogon like peptide 1, ozampic, manjaro and similar drugs

my understanding of the back history on the isa that a biologist obsessed with Helomonsters a reptile that doesn't need to eat very often discovered a peptide within their bloodstream called extended yeah that allowed them to eat very seldom, it curbed appetite in the Helomonsters of all things

and it has an analog homologue you know we don't know I don't know the sequence homology exactly but there's a similar peptide made in mice and in humans that suppresses appetite if you would could you tell us what is known about how GLP1 works to suppress appetite where in the body end or brain and your sort of read of these drugs and what's happening there good, bad, exciting, ugly, happy to anything else so the story of GLP1, so the Helomonsters an important turn and I'll talk about that

it actually goes back before that quite a ways so I should take a step back and say you know these were developed as drugs for diabetes and diabetes is a condition where basically a elevated blood glucose either because you don't produce enough insulin or because your insulin is not effective and so back in sort of the 1920s right around the time insulin was discovered there is this phenomenon on discovery known as the Increotin effect

and what it was in Creotin? In Creotin, not the Creotin effect not the Creotin effect, in numerous places in daily life and online just kidding so it's called the Increotin effect, you can think of it as increase insulin because that's what the effect is

and the idea was that if you take glucose by mouth if you consume glucose orally versus if you have the same amount of glucose injected intravenously more insulin is produced when you take the glucose orally versus if it's delivered intravenously

suggesting something about the process of ingesting the glucose causes more insulin to be released and causes you to lower your body sugar more accurately and more strongly, interesting which is a little bit counterintuitive because in the pancreas right

so insulin is released from the pancreas from the beta cell the pancreas senses the glucose concentration in the blood directly and so it's just that insulin is being released not just in response to changes in blood glucose but in response to a second factor and so they call that an Increotin

and through various experiments it was it was shown that this Increotin effect comes from the intestine that there's some substance being produced by the intestine that when you eat a meal of sugar goes through your intestine that boosts this insulin response to glucose in the blood

and people immediately realize this could potentially be very valuable and the reason is that you know you can treat diabetes with insulin injections but insulin is dangerous right because if you inject too much insulin you can kill yourself I'm making yourself hypoglycemic right so this is to be very careful but the thing about the Increotin effect is it's not causing insulin release directly but it's rather boosting the natural insulin release that comes when glucose is higher in your blood

so it's sort of an amplifier on the natural insulin release so basically in the years that follow whenever someone would find a new hormone they would test it is it this Increotin and there's lots of failures they weren't the Increotin

but then so there's other hormone that comes from the pancreas called glucagon and so glucagon is also discovered in the 1920s glucagon is kind of the anti-insulin so when blood sugar goes low glucagon is released in order to cause your liver to release glucose into the blood

so glucagon and insulin are these two opposing hormones glucagon was known for a long time but but but people discovered in certain 1980s that the glucagon gene is expressed in other tissues other than the pancreas and it's differentially processed the protein is differentially processed

to produce different hormones hormones other than glucagon and they discovered there was one in the intestine and so they called it glucagon like peptide because it came from the same gene but it's just slightly different it's cut up slightly differently and this hormone wasn't in creedin

so basically if you put it on beta cells you get this increased response of insulin in response to glucose and so there was the idea okay this could be a great diabetes drug but I should say there is one other in creedin that's that's been found

it's called GIP and that will be important to talk about some of these other drugs also a hormone that comes from the intestine and so the challenge with making GLP1 into a drug is that as an extremely short half-life so it has a half-life about two minutes in the blood

and so even if you inject people with GLP1 it won't really be useful for anything you don't decrease appetite but it sure has just degraded too fast and the reason is degraded is because there's an enzyme DPP4 is what it's called that degrades GLP1

so the first thing people tried was let's make inhibitors of that enzyme so we can boost this natural GLP1 signal and those are approved diabetes drugs are called glipidins you've probably heard about them genuvias the most common one and those boost the level of GLP1

the natural GLP1 and produce from the intestine by about threefold and they're effective in treating diabetes that is if people lose weight people do not lose weight and that's one of the key reasons that we know the natural function of GLP1 is not really to control body weight

because you can boost the level threefold with these DPP4 drugs millions of people have taken them they do not lose weight that's a great question so but you know a threefold is great but like you'd like to increase it even more and to do that you can't block this enzyme

you have to actually produce a GLP1 that is more stable in the blood that's where this lizard that you're talking you were mentioned comes into play it produces a stabilized form of GLP1 and it's a venom no one knows why one hypothesis is that it's something to do with the lizard

as you said basically having this long time period between meals and it needs to regulate its blood glucose who knows if that's true but it turned out to be fortuitous because then this GLP1 from this lizard it has a half life of like two hours and so the first GLP1 drug that was approved

was just this molecule from this lizard basically and it's called a exenetide as approved in 2005 works well for diabetes has a half life of two hours you inject it and doesn't cause a ton of weight loss but two hours is good but it's not so great so then pharmaceutical industry

can we you know basically improve this even further and so start engineering this hormone making mutations, attaching lipid tails to make it buy into this proteins in the blood that would stabilize it chemistry jockey stuff yeah exactly and I think the next big advance was this compound lyraglutide

and lyraglutide was approved for diabetes in 2010 and then for weight loss in 2014 and so lyraglutide has a half life of about 13 hours in the blood loss now you're getting up to something serious we've gone from two minutes, two hours, 13 hours and you get better effects on aspects of blood glucose

and diabetes control and they started to see that some people were losing weight very variable response it's not everyone loses weight on lyraglutide and one of the things they noticed that I think is just as fascinating sort of example of how drug discovery works in the real world

you know a lot of these people would take lyraglutide now it has this longer half life they'll start to get nauseous and that would limit how much of the lyraglutide they could take and it's a known side effect with the GLP1 drugs it causes nausea and sort of this gastrointestinal distress

but they notice that over time the nausea would just sort of go away and so they would start dose escalating sort of raising the dose that the person would take so you would go, you know, a month at this dose and then a month at a slightly higher dose and then a month at a slightly higher dose

and you could work your way up and these side effects would reappear but then they go away and then once you got after the highest dose then people really started losing weight and so there's a couple of things that are pharmaceutical industry

realized that these are potentially really effective weight loss drugs and also this nausea which we thought was, you know, a killer people are able to just get used to it and then it just goes away and it goes, the worst, tackle-flaxis so the idea is that the receptor that's affecting the gut

that's causing these effects it undergoes some sort of downregulation with chronic exposure so lyraglutide, you know, has been around, you know, has been on the market for 14 years now was used but still you're only getting sort of like seven to 10% weight loss which is good

but not like, you know, amazing, impressive but then semaglutide came along and that was approved for diabetes in 2017 and semaglutide is ozemic or also marketed as wig-o-v for weight loss and semaglutide now has a half-life of seven days

so now we've gone from two minutes, two hours, 13 hours, seven days and you can really jack up the concentration with a seven-day half-life and then they saw people start really losing weight and so and some of those trials people lost, you know, 16% of their body weight which previously had been unattainable, right? in what time frame?

typically takes about a year and most of the loss in body weight is from body fat or from other compartments the typical number is that if you lose weight either through dieting or through taking one of these drugs and you don't do anything like eat a high-protein diet or do resistance training

somewhere between 25 and 33% of what you lose is going to be muscle the rest is going to be fat but as you said, some of that could be offset by resistance training and or consuming a higher protein diet you can almost completely eliminate that if you eat enough protein and do serious weightlifting

obviously not the whole population is interested in doing that and there's been a lot of discussion of how serious the side effect this is among elderly people you don't want to be losing muscle mass because you're already losing so much muscle mass

on the other hand, the counter-argument that's been made which I think is also kind of convincing is that true, you're losing some muscle but you're also losing all this fat and you no longer need as much muscle when you're not carrying around as much body fat

so people who are heavier naturally have more muscle because they need to move their body and so the calves on very obese people are often enormous exactly and then they lose weight and I mentioned the calves in particular because they're carrying a lot of the body load exactly exactly

so it's still an open question as to whether how serious a problem this muscle-lead muscle mass loss is although pharmaceutical industry is all in now on making drugs that basically are going to prevent that so that's something that will be happening probably in the future

is it a sorry to interrupt but is the weight loss on these drugs the consequence of reduced appetite or some other aspect of metabolism and if it's the consequence of reduced appetite is that occurring at the level of the brain and gut or combination so it's almost entirely reduced appetite

and it's almost entirely incurring at the level of the brain which neurons it's thought that the key targets of these drugs are neurons in these two regions one's called the nucleus of the solitary tract and the other one's called the area post-traumatic so we're back in the brainstem

so these are actually the neurons in that deserabat rat story I was telling earlier these are the brain regions that are preserved in the deserabat rat the deserabat still has these very caudal brainstem structures they're two very special brain regions

because they get direct input from the vagus nerve so the vagus nerve is the nerve that innervates your stomach and intestines and heart and lungs it's sort of the major pathway from gut to brain and provides most of the sensory or the neural input

from gut to brain telling you about things like this your stomach distension how many nutrients are in your intestine breathing all that stuff and almost all of those vagal nerves terminate on these two structures in the brainstem when I hear post-trauma I think about nausea

because I was taught that post-trauma contains neurons that can stimulate vomiting and this seems to link up well at least in the logical sense with the idea that stimulating, activating receptors in these neurons within post-trauma might explain part of the transient nausea side effect it does

of those amput and related drugs yeah so the current thought is that a lot of the nausea is coming from activating the neurons of the area post-trauma and that a lot of the sort of physiologic satiety is coming from activating the neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract

now the whole brain is connected to each other and so if you really turn on these neurons in the NTS and the AP they're going to talk to the hypothalamus and all these other brain regions it's going to change the whole brain so it's not just those regions

but these drugs don't have great access to the brain they can penetrate a little bit into the brain but they don't penetrate into the whole brain and it's thought that if you take fluorescently labeled versions of these drugs and see where they so you can visualize where they actually go

and they're enriched in these structures in the brain stem so that's why people think that this is probably where they're acting and is that because there are there's an abundance of the receptors for these compounds in post-trauma and NTS or is it

because the blood brain barrier is somehow weaker at that location it's because the blood brain barrier is weaker so basically it's a region what's known as a circumventricular organ meaning it's one of these rare places in the brain where the blood brain barrier is weakened

and so substances can come from the outside into the brain and that's important for these big peptides because these are not small molecules these are big peptides with lipid chains on them and other things and so they can really get only get into areas

of the brain where the blood brain barrier is weakened I really appreciate that you mentioned the half-life issue with GLP1 and the fact that these DPP4 and antagonists did not lead to weight loss despite increasing circulating GLP by threefold this is relevant to a number of different claims

that people make that a given food or a given drink increases GLP1 I've actually said before, you know, I'm a big consumer of your vermote my father's side is Argentine and it's a known appetite suppressant but it contains caffeine and other stimulants that might explain some of that

and it's not a robust appetite suppressant to the point where most people would rely on it as a weight loss compound but anyway, it's my preferred source of caffeine but I've said before, you know, there's some evidence that it can increase GLP1 but based on what you've said

the increases in GLP1 that it creates are very unlikely to produce the kind of appetite-suppressive effect that would lead to any significant weight loss in somebody that's obese presumably that are separate from any caffeine stimulatoria. So you can't separate because it's a complex compound this year of the Montet thing.

It's got lots of things in it but also, you know, I've observed you being vocal on social media when people have said, hey, this thing increases GLP1 quite appropriately, I think, said, wait, you know, ozemic and drugs like that increase GLP1, thousandfold

when you talk about a food or drink or maybe a supplement increasing GLP1, it's very unlikely to increase GLP1 to that level meaning unless you're getting into the hundredfold or thousandfold increases probably not right to talk about GLP1 being the source of any appetite-suppressive effect. Yeah, that's all correct. So, I mean, I think it's important sometimes to distinguish between pharmacologic and physiologic effects.

So physiologic is what the hormone naturally does in your body and what can be modulated by natural things like eating a different food. And you might get a twofold change in your GLP1 by eating a different food, you know, one food versus the other. But as we know from those DPP4 inhibitors, it's not going to really change your appetite because the drugs increase at threefold. These GLP1 agonists are really a pharmacologic effect. In fact, it only happens with drugs.

So you get a thousand to ten thousandfold higher concentrations of these drugs in your blood than the natural hormone. And so there's no diet that's ever going to give you that. And there's no precedent for it either. So should we be at all concerned about that? I mean, they run clinical trials and address safety.

But when you're talking about a thousandfold increase in essentially a peptide hormone, if we were talking about different peptide hormone, not you know, pick one, you know, oxytocin or estrogen testosterone, they're not really broadly speaking. Most people would be concerned about thousandfold dancing of something like that. And obviously there are clinical indications where that's important.

However, my observation of the ever expanding literature on GLP1 agonists is that there seems to be improvements in like reduction in alcohol consumption. And by the way, why would increasing GLP1 reduce craving for alcohol? It seems like there's an ever expanding list of things that GLP1 agonism is good for. But we are talking about soup. I would say super physiological level. That's when one takes it. And again, I'm not against it. More for it. I'm just paying attention to the literature.

So I would say that that's absolutely right. When you're increasing level of hormone a thousandfold, you need to be careful and see what's happening. But at the end, it's an empirical question. What does it actually do to a person? And it can only be answered through experiments. And I think the nice thing about these GLP1 drugs that a lot of people don't realize is they've been around and improved since 2005, the earliest ones.

And even something like ozempic, which may be only entered the public consciousness in the last year or two, right? It's been around for seven-ish years, I think. So and big clinical trials with these drugs. And so and the evidence so far is that they seem to be incredibly safe. And as you said, not just incredibly safe, but they seem to have all these unexpected health benefits that seem to be in some cases even unrelated to weight loss.

And so because of the reasons you mentioned, one of the things the FDA requires from these pharmaceutical companies for diabetes drugs is these large cardiac outcome trials. So basically where you measure stroke and where you measure heart attacks and death from any cardiac cause, big trials, like 20,000 people, four years, cost like a billion dollars to run. And the data from the semiglutide, the ozempic trial, came out last year.

And as expected, reduced the rate of heart attacks, strokes, all caused mortality according to cardiac reasons. But what was really surprising was a lot of that seemed to happen before the people even lose weight, lost weight. So there was already a difference between the placebo group and the semiglutide group before the people on the drug had lost a significant amount of weight.

And there was no correlation between the amount of weight they lost and how well they were protected from heart disease. And that's why many people think that some of these effects actually could be due to other things that GLP ones are doing that we didn't expect. And so one thing is there's an idea emerging that they are anti-inflammatory.

So these brain regions, they are a post-stream in the NTS, are also really important for this reflex known as the inflammatory reflex that basically acts and starts with the vagus nerve, goes to these brain regions in the brain stem, and goes back down to the body to basically suppress, to prevent out of control inflammation. And so it's thought that these drugs perhaps have an anti-inflammatory effect that explains some of that.

Sounds like the patent on these drugs just got extended by another hundred years. That's a bio-farm joke. I mean, just to put context on it, drugs can be patented and sold as a commercial version and not as generic versions until the patent runs out. Unless companies are able to find another approved clinical use, in which case it can be remarketed only as a brand name, not generic version.

So a lot of companies, once they do the safety testing and all given everything they put into the R&D, into the research and development, there's a very big incentive to not necessarily finding new drugs, but finding new uses for the same drugs, and not allowing generic versions into the picture. And that's why it's likely to be based on these, what sounds like additional uses of ozemic-related compounds a long time before there's generic ozemic available. I think it will be a while.

I don't know the exact status of the patents, but I'm guessing it's going to be a while before there are generic versions, but there's a lot of competition coming. So every major pharmaceutical company, or almost every major pharmaceutical company, now has a GLP-1 program. And some of them are really exciting, actually. So, I mean, the general trend in this area is what people call GLP-1+.

Which means you take the GLP-1 agonist, which is already giving you 15% weight loss or so, and then you add additional things to that to give it additional properties. So one compound is from Eli Lilly, which makes this other drug on the market that we haven't talked about, but Terzepatide, which is known as Muncharo, for diabetes and the Zepabound for obesity, which is even better, really, in almost every respected, better drug than OZMPIC.

So people lose more weight since about 21% weight loss at a year. Fewer side effects, at least at comparable doses. That seems to be because this other drug, Terzepatide, it has two targets, not one. So whereas OZMPIC is just GLP-1 receptor agonist, Terzepatide is a dual agonist of GLP-1 and this other incretion that we talked about, GIP. And it seems like having that GIP agonism actually acts as an anti-nauzia effect.

The sort of counteract some of the nausea caused by the GLP-1 in the area post-streamer. There are a GIP receptor neurons in the area post-streamer of the Snauzia Center. So it sort of allows you to crank up the dose of the GLP-1 agonism even further while you're suppressing the nausea and just get even more weight loss. So now, talking about the future, things that aren't available yet, but will be in the next couple of years.

So Eli Lilly, the company that makes the Strog Terzepatide and Slash Mujaro, they have a triple agonist that's in phase three clinical trials now. So this is now three hormones in one. It's the GLP-1, which all the Strogs have, the GIP, which is the anti-nauzia component, and then GLG-1 itself. And so these three hormones all combine in one pill. And what the GLG-1 does is it increases energy expenditure. And this is a well-known effective GLG-1. And so you're basically eating less.

Your nausea isn't as bad. And now you're just burning more calories at baseline. And the results from the Strog are incredible. So basically, there's been one phase two trial published, and people lost 25% of their body weight at the end of the, I think, was 48-week period, and they were still losing weight. So we don't know where the end point. We don't know what the maximum is. So they're bigger, longer trials going on now to figure that out.

But at that point, when you get beyond 25% body weight, you're talking about, basically, bariatric surgery, right? Which is currently the best thing we have, you know, like these surgeries people do to stomach states. We're moving a portion of the stomach. We're moving a portion of the gut. So really, it's a pharmacologic version of bariatric surgery. The other one that I think is really exciting, there's this compound from Amgen. It's called, it's just right now, it's just a code.

It's like AMG-133. But it's like, there's appetite. In the sense that targets both GLP1 and GIP. So it's a dual targeted. But unlike tersepotide, which activates the GIP receptor, this Amgen compound inhibits it. And for reasons that people don't understand, either activating or inhibiting this receptor causes you to lose weight. So still a mystery, but a lot of debate about what's going on there.

But the way this Amgen compound activates the GIP receptor, or inhibits the GIP receptor rather, is that it's an antibody. So all these other things were peptides. But this is a much bigger sexual protein. This is an antibody. And because it's an antibody, it has a much longer lifetime, even than something like stomach lootide, which is seven days, so it's like a month, and blood or something. And so you can give people monthly injections of this, and they lose dramatic amounts of weight.

And then at least in this initial trial, at the end of this, they stopped, and people maintained the weight loss for six months. That's impressive. Potentially because of the long lasting effects of this antibody, or potentially because of other things that we don't understand. So, and those are just two. There's all sorts of other crazy things happening. So really, I think it's just created this explosion of interest in pharma. Basically, it's one of these things.

You know, once you see that something can be done, all of a sudden, that changes everyone's perspective. And so now, obesity drug discovery has gone from something that 10 years ago, everyone wanted to stay away from because there were so many nightmare stories about drugs that turned out to be not safe. Till now, everybody's sort of all in on this. Yeah, remembering college, the FENFEN debacle, where a diet drug was released, and people had cardiac issues start dying.

So it was pulled from market, and then it was essentially a quiet field for a long time. In part to bring us back into the brain, and in part because it's directly relevant to what we've been discussing about ozemic and GLP1. There are other neurons in the brain that regulate feeding. And there are other peptides involved in appetite control for which I would say niche communities have started to indulge in.

And by the way, people were taking GLP1 analogs long before they were FDA approved in kind of niche communities. These aren't communities I'm a part of, but everyone's a while sticking here into one of these communities and hear what people are taking and a big thing right now in these communities is the use of other peptides that are in the melanocyte stimulating hormone pathway. And you mentioned melanocortican receptor containing neurons.

Could you tell us a little bit about what these neurons do in the absence of any pharmacologic stimulation? And then why it would be that people would perhaps stimulate these pathways with these drugs, not that we're recommending them.

But I do think that given that some of these neurons are also involved in sexual behavior and FDA approved for the treatment of hyposexual function in women, things like that, there is FDA approval for some of these compounds that they're interesting hypothalamic neurons that are starting to gain more attention and that I predict based on their potential involvement in feeding appetite and weight control are likely to enter the picture with more prominence in the not too distant future.

So alpha-MSH, a scientist called the hormone you were just referring to, is a product of pumps, the POMC gene. So in the same way that we just talked about glucagon can be processed into different things as some gene and some cells that's made into the glucagon hormone and other cells that's made into GOP1. POMC, that gene can be processed to prove different hormones. And one is alpha-MSH, which is very important for feeding control.

And so these POMC neurons are in the arqueotinucleus of the hypothalamus, the same region where these agiar penurons I talked about earlier are located. And there's sort of these two sets of neurons have opposing effects on body weight regulation. And so alpha-MSH inhibits food intake and agiar penurons promote food intake. And where they converge is at this receptor of the melanocortin-4 receptor, which is important for body weight regulation. And so alpha-MSH is an agonist.

It turns on that receptor. And the agarpeapaphtide is an antagonist. It turns it off. And so, you know, there's a lot of human genetics, as I mentioned earlier, implicating this pathway in body weight regulation. There have been a lot of efforts over many years to turn alpha-MSH into a drug. And it's been very difficult. There is one drug that's now approved. It's called, I think, I'm going to get the name wrong. It's like a set melanatide or something like this. It's an MC4 receptor agonist.

It's mainly used in relatively small populations of people that, for example, have mutations in this pathway. It's not used as a widespread as a drug. And the challenge has been really side effects. So there's an increase in blood pressure that happens sometimes with these medicines partly because this pathway controls not only appetite, but also autonomic tone and sympathetic nervous system activation. So it's just taking a step back from everything we've talked about today.

I talked about this short-term system and the long-term system, the control's energy balance and body weight, the long-s short-term system and the brain stem, the long-term system and the hypothalamus, the long-term system being leptin and alpha-MSH and agRP. When I was coming up, learning about this stuff, 15 years ago or 20 years ago, you know, the dogma was, you could only affect body weight through the long-term system, by manipulating the long-term system.

Because any manipulation you did of the short-term system in the brain stem, the animal would just compensate. And there were these famous experiments where they would take CCK, which is a hormone just like GLP1, injected into rats, injected several times a day, and CCK is known to decrease the size of meals, and it would decrease the size of meals.

But the rats would never lose any weight because they would just eat more meals to compensate and they would just perfectly compensate, but eating more meals. And so the lore was, it's just impossible. It's compensated unless you hit this body weight set point regulating area, which is the hypothalamus, the long-term system.

But then with the pharmaceutical industry discovered, which I guess maybe shouldn't be so surprising, but I guess it was to some people, is that if you just hit that receptor, that short-term system 24 hours a day, seven days a week, and never let it stop, then you will lose weight, right? And so the short-term system alone is enough to cause body weight regulation.

On the other hand, the long-term system with alpha-MSH and agRP neurons and pumps in all this stuff has been a challenge to pharmaceutical target. Because you know, leptin, we discussed, didn't really work. And so I think there's going to be, as you mentioned, a reemergence of interest in considering this other pathway now that we've seen the success of the GLP ones. And I think one area where it may emerge is in considering their combination, perhaps at different stages of weight loss.

So perhaps, you know, what would make a lot of sense scientifically, I don't know if it'll work in practice, but I think it'll take a GLP one drug to lose the weight. And then at some point you might stop that drug and switch to a more hypothalamus center, leptin-based drug to keep the weight off. So basically, use the GLP one drug to force yourself to lose the weight and then use the leptin hypothalamus-based drug to sort of say, okay, this is our new body weight set point.

Let's not resist this weight loss that's happened. Whether that will actually make sense practically is hard to say because you know, the GLP one drug has just a lot of benefits even beyond weight loss so people might not want to stop taking them. But that's one idea. Very interesting. I'd love to talk about dopamine. Sure. We hear so much about dopamine being involved in pleasure.

I like to think I've had at least a small level of impact in convincing people that it's also involved in perhaps mostly involved in things like motivation, different forms of learning, and lots of other things too folks. Dopamine does lots of things. It's even expressed in the eye and controls adaptation to light. So it does lots of things. But it certainly is believed that dopamine is involved in our either craving for food or pleasure from food.

What's the real story on dopamine as it relates to food and eating behavior? You had a beautiful paper published in Nature entitled, and we'll put a link to this in the show note captions. Dopamine subsystems that track internal states. And I love this paper for a variety of reasons. If you could give us the high points of your discoveries on dopamine as it relates to feeding, I think I know in fact that people would find it very illuminating. Sure. Fantastic.

So yeah, the question of what dopamine does with respect to feeding is a great question and a difficult question, I think, to answer. There's a lot of misconceptions. I think the evidence is dopamine probably isn't so much involved in the pleasure of food. That tastes the hedonic experience. One reason we think this is because you can make mice, the Richard Paul-Mutter did this decades ago, that don't have any dopamine. And they still show the same sort of effective responses to foods.

He puts something sweet in their mouth that kind of, they like it, right? What dopamine seems to be important for with respect to food is two things. One is the motivation to engage in work to get food, particularly when it's high levels of effort. So if you ask a mouse to press a lever to get a pellet of food, if it doesn't have any dopamine, it won't do it. And if it has low levels of dopamine, it'll just work a little bit.

So dopamine is important for sort of energizing action and motivating you to engage in hard tasks. The other thing that dopamine is really important for is learning. And it's important for learning about which cues predict something useful for the body and feeding is a central example of that. And what that paper of ours is about is the idea that this learning actually happens on two different time scales for two different kinds of cues.

So what we almost always talk about with dopamine and learning, which is important, is learning about how external cues in the environment predict something like food availability, right? So you see a McDonald's sign and you know that that means there's some tasty food in there. And so dopamine is involved in that process of sort of learning what that external cue means. And that's a very fast time scale process.

So in the laboratory, for example, we will play a tone and then give an animal a sip of a solution that has calories in it, for example. And it can learn the association between that tone and that the food is going to be available if they're separated by a few seconds, but that's all. And that's a dopamine dependent process.

But there's a second sort of much slower time scale learning about food, which isn't about where I go to get hamburger, but rather about what the experience of eating the food, the oro sensory experience, its taste, its flavor, its texture, how that relates to the post-injustive effects. And I should say that this seems extremely relevant to the McDonald's example, because in your experimental situation, the tone is analogous to the golden arches of the McDonald's sign. Exactly.

But in my experience, and forgive me, but most of the food that I've consumed from McDonald's does not taste good. Relative to other really delicious hamburgers or french fries or something like that. I mean, it's so you're saying dopamine is required to link the signal, the golden arches or the tone to the presence of food at a particular location. Exactly. But not to the experience of pleasure from that food. Exactly. Which squares very well with my experience of McDonald's.

And I probably haven't had that bite of McDonald's in 20 plus years. Yeah, I would have to be pretty hungry. I haven't either. And it's funny, the golden arches thing is just something that people in neuroscience talk about dopamine use. And so now I've started subconsciously just talking about golden arches, even though I also haven't eaten it in McDonald's in decades. In and out burger, the better tasting from what I understand, probably better sourcing.

We're not going to get into all this in detail, but everyone has their preferences. But I do think it's interesting, because what we're talking about here is a related, I think, to this notion of highly processed food, packaging, the commodization of food. Which is the idea that we are drawn to food for things other than the taste that we expect for. There's all this context. That's right. So I think an important distinction that people make is its distinction between wanting and liking.

I don't know if you've talked about this previously on the podcast. On a Lemke, my colleague at Stanford came on the podcast talked about dopamine is about wanting as opposed to enjoying. Exactly. In most cases. So liking is the subjective, hedonic pleasure in the moment of eating it, but wanting is just what you want. And this can be uncoupled all the time. You can want things that at the end of the day, you don't actually enjoy it when you get it. I feel like a lot of life is like that.

Indeed. And so dopamine is very powerful in making you want something, but not necessarily like it. So that's one element. But then there's this other element that is important, but very much less studied. But I find much more interesting, which is how you connect the sensory cues associated with food, its taste, its flavor, its smell, with the consequences for the body.

And this is so important because so much of whether we like or dislike a particular food or drink is related to its post-injustive effects. You come to like things, for example, that are calories. So this is one of the reasons that adults will eat vegetables and other savory foods that children find disgusting. Even though they're a little bit bitter, you learn through experience, this makes me feel good to eat this.

And even maybe at a completely subconscious level, there's also a level of learning that occurs. And this of course happens with other things like coffee and beer and other things like that. And so there's been an idea that this other much slower learning occurs. And the reason I say it's slower is because the time between when you taste the food and when it actually gets into your intestine and releases the hormones that my drive, this is quite slow, separated by tens of minutes.

But how that works hasn't been clear. There's been an idea that dopamine might be involved, but it hadn't really received a lot of attention. And so we set out to investigate what is the role of dopamine in these post-injustive responses and sort of map out for the dopamine system. How does the dopamine system respond?

Not when you see the golden arches, which is usually the kinds of experiments that have been performed, but rather when you deliver nutrients directly to your stomach, or when you deliver water directly to your stomach if you're thirsty and so on. And what we saw was that there are these different populations of dopamine neurons that are tuned to respond to signals from inside the body. And so there are some that respond when nutrients are in the stomach and intestine.

There are others that respond when in a thirsty mouse, when the blood is rehydrated, when you basically say, shade your thirst. And we showed that the purpose, or at least a purpose of that activation, is to cause you to learn about the effects of what you just ate. Basically to create this connection between the flavor of something and its post-injustive effects.

That sort of the delayed dopamine signal after ingested food and fluids is sort of reinforcing this connection between the flavor of what I just ate and that it was something good for me. One of the sort of interesting things about that paper that was not the direction we initially expected to go in is that for food I think it's kind of intuitive. There are lots of flavors to food. You have to learn what all these different flavors mean.

For thirst people find it a little less obvious because thirst is just water. Aren't you just born knowing what water is? Like how do you have to learn anything to do with drinking a glass of water? But it actually is a learning question in part because for many animals, probably most animals. Thirst is something that's associated with eating, not drinking. There's a study I love of rabbits in New Zealand.

So there's not a lot of people studying what animals, how they get their fluids in the wild, because who cares. But it's kind of interesting. And so in New Zealand, there's a huge rabbit problem because they're invasive pests species that was introduced in the 1800s and they're just eating all the land. So there's lots of money to study rabbits to understand their ecology.

And so group of researchers did this experiment where they made this big pen outside where they put a bunch of rabbits in this. The rabbits couldn't escape, but they had all their natural food. It was like an outdoor area. And they also put a trough of water. So the rats always had access to water, which is a clean water. And they could measure how much water the rabbits drank. And what they basically found is that nine months out of a year, rabbits drank zero water.

They drink absolutely zero because they get all of their water from food. The only time they drink is during the winter when all of the greenery has sort of become shriveled and then they can't get water from that anymore. And so it's just kind of interesting aspect of how many animals are very different from the way we think about ingestive behavior. But that fact that animals have to get water from food raises this question, how do they know which foods are rehydrating?

That's presumably they have to learn that because you can't just look at a food and say if you've never had any experience. Oh yeah, this is something that's very water rich and this will rehydrate me when I'm thirsty and this one is not. And so James, the graduate student who led this project, basically investigated this by giving mice different fluids and then measuring how the dopamine responds. And he showed that there was this delayed dopamine response after the mice had drank the fluids.

The correlated with rehydration of the blood. So a whole bunch of dopamine neurons get strongly activated when the blood is rehydrated. And he hypothesized this might be a signal, this delayed activation of dopamine neurons that allows animals in the wild to learn that food I just ate is rehydrating. And so he did an experiment where he basically gave them two different flavors mimicking sort of the flavors of two different foods, one of which was hydrating and one of which was not.

And the animals couldn't tell because he infused the water directly into their stomach. And he showed that basically these dopamine neurons are critical for them learning that association. So that's the story of that. I love it and I'll tell you why. When I was in college for reasons that I don't recall, I decided to run an experiment on myself where I would eat one meal that was fairly low water content, like a piece of meat or something with some cheese.

You know what some people call a keto meal, but I wasn't ketogenic. I don't even think I knew what a ketogenic diet was at that point. And then the next meal I would have like a salad and some fruits. And then I would switch back and forth. And I generally would only two or three times a day, you know, anyway, there's only so many hours in the day. And I found it to be incredibly satiating. And I found that I felt great.

And I can imagine any number of different reasons for that. And there are these theories that you probably recall the diet that was being promoted in the 90s where people eat either carbohydrates or protein separately. Like there was a whackingness out there. And as I say that, I'm sure I'll get assaulted in the comments. It's probably not wacky. I'm sure there's some enzymatic basis for why that would be useful. If you enjoy it, go for it.

You know, I don't have a feeling about it one way or the other. But one thing I noticed was that low water content containing meals either by virtue of the foods that they include or by virtue of the fact that they're not diluted, so to speak. It's a different taste experience to eat those foods than it is to eat like a big salad or something of that sort. In any event, I don't do that any longer. I just sort of stopped, but it was a fun experiment.

And I think it was efficient because at the time I had very low money as a student. So, you know, generally fruits and vegetables were less costly than meats and things that sort. But in all seriousness, to what extent do you think humans overeat or under eat depending on the water content of the food? It's an interesting question. So, you know, there is this advice that you should, if you're hungry, first drink something, drink some water and see if you're still hungry.

And the idea is that perhaps humans can't always, or we are inter-oceptive sense or our ability to sense what our body needs is not perfect. And sometimes we could be confused and we could really be thirsty when we're hungry and hungry when we're thirsty. And there's some evidence that could help. I would say it's probably not a huge effect in most of modern day life, but it's an interesting idea.

Yeah. Yeah. This brings us to the topic of thirst, something that your laboratories worked on extensively, and the topic of osmolarity. Yeah, of salt consumption and things of that sort. In broad terms, how do these things link up? Meaning, are there instances in which what we really need is salt and we end up eating a bunch of Parmesan cheese.

I got teased yesterday by my team because occasionally when I'm on the road, I don't like most of the foods available in most airports and stuff. So I'll bring a chunk of really nice Parmesan cheese. I'll break off a piece and eat it. I'll have half a cucumber and I'll have a can of, not a can of tuna, but there are these wonderful, a jarred filet of tunas that are available that are in olive oil. They taste really good.

They're not canned tuna. It's really good. And I'd rather eat that in most cases until I can get to a decent meal than what's put in front of me on an airplane most of the time. So I get teased about this, but I noticed that for instance, sometimes I'll eat the cheese and I think, oh, actually what I really just want is the salt. Yeah.

I really want salt. I've been drinking a lot of coffee today. I've had a couple extra glasses of water. Maybe I'm just craving salt. And I'm confused and I'm over consuming this cheese. Yes. When in fact what I'm going for is the salt. As you point out, our understanding of exactly what we need is fairly crude and oftentimes we overshoot the margin, especially when foods are in combination.

So salt, water, and let's just say calories. How do we accurately or inaccurately pursue those at the level of biology? Okay. So I was drawing tough questions. I know. But you're, you're, you're, it feels like my qualifying example. So, so well, there are separate system, this ought to be separate systems that control salt appetite thirst for water and hunger for calories. And so they're involved different brain regions for the most part different neurons, different signals from the body.

In general, hunger and thirst are pretty separable. The, I would say the, the instance where they interact is in phenomena such as dehydration and erexia. This is the idea that if I give you some dry food, but I don't give you any water, event you're going to eat less food because basically you're going to get dehydrated and you're going to decide I need to preserve my, my fluid balance, even if I eat less calories.

So we prioritize hydration. Yes, you will at some point you will prioritize hydration that's related also the concept of brand deal drinking. So many animals, including humans drink most of their water during meals because you basically want to counteract the osmolyze that are in your food.

Salt balance though and in thirst, so the thirst for water and the desire for salt are much more tightly linked because the purpose of both systems is to maintain the composition of the blood at its right concentration. So you, you want to have the right osmolyze of the blood, which you can just think of in simple terms as sort of the total concentration of all the salts.

It's a little more complicated than that, but it doesn't really matter. And you also specifically need to maintain the sodium concentration at the right level. And so, and there are really powerful innate mechanisms that drive both. I think thirst is very intuitive to people. You get dehydrated, you lose water, you become a thirsty.

And we know now that that they're very small set of neurons in few brain regions that control that. And the way they they're thought to work is they contain osmosensors. So they contain same basically these neurons are sensors for the osmolality of the blood and they're activated when the blood osmolality gets too high. And it's incredibly sensitive system. So, so you can perceive an increase in your blood osmolality of 1% as the sensation of thirst. So, remarkable.

Yeah. That's how critical it is to maintain salt balance. Exactly. Exactly. And so, you know, you get to 10% increase in blood osmolality and you're in extreme discomfort and 20% you're like in the hospital. So, if I took a let's just say a half an ounce sip of seawater inadvertently. Yes. It's extremely aversive. It is. It's like, like you just you you want to drink some non salty water. So, nice clean water. Yes. Exactly. Immediately.

Yeah. So, I should emphasize that there's two components to the fluid homeostasis system for the water homeostasis system. One is this desire to drink. But the other is of course the kidney. And so, the reason the drinking the salt water won't put you in a really bad situation is your kidney would then filter out a lot of that salt and cause you just to pee it out and then you'll be fine.

So, those two work in balance the kid is controlling how much of the salt gets reabsorbed into the blood and then this desire for thirst this desire to drink allow you to replenish the blood with water at various intervals.

And so, yeah, I mean the experiments led to discovery this third circuitry are amazing is this guy bank Anderson working in the 1950s and he just had this hypothesis that there was an osmosensor in the brain, right, which is very I think there were some evidence suggested but it was not really really strongly supported at the time by the data.

And so, he took these goats and he just started infusing small amounts of salt into various places in their brain, reasoning that if there was an osmosensor. I mean, I wasn't chuckling at it just like it like I you know, I feel for the goats I feel for everyone involved in the experiment but what a wild experiment just to put salt directly into the brain.

So, the concentrated saline solution yeah, and he found this tiny region in and around the hypothalamus that if you if you have few salt in this region the goats will drink like eight liters of water in five minutes just crazy right. And then he and so he reason, okay, this must be the osmosensor and then he went back and stimulated those neurons is just the same thing the goat just drinks like crazy.

And so now we notice this this couple small regions in the environment hypothalamus once called the sub four-nacle organ and other ones called what doesn't really matter but basically that have these osmosensors.

One of the interesting things about the regulation of fluid balance is you face some of the same challenges we just talked about with the regulation of food consumption which is that you have this behavior this ingestive behavior that leads to replenishment of the body but there are these delays right.

So, if you're thirsty and you drink a glass of water it can take on the order of sort of 20 to 30 minutes for the water to be absorbed into your blood for the blood to be rehydrated and then for these osmosensors that banked Anderson discovered in your brain to be to be sort of sense that in return to normal activity. But of course, if you had the experience of drinking a glass of water you know that you can quench your thirst within minutes right. So, how does that work within seconds even.

One of the others what was sort of experiments we did early in my lab was to to ask that question by basically recording for the first time the activity of these neurons that banked and discovered by putting the salt the goes we went back into them now in mice mice of the same neurons you have the same neurons and recording their activity when a thirsty mice drinks and asks what happens.

And what we saw was that the neurons don't wait until the blood is is rehydrated they also don't do what the agirp neurons do is meaning they don't look at the water and predict how much water they're going to drink. But instead they get a signal from the mouth which every time the mouse takes a lick of water their activity goes down a little bit and basically they track in that way the volume of water that's passed through the mouth.

They also get the signal from the blood but really relaying the osmolarity of the blood and they compare these two and basically when the mouse is drunk enough in order to. For the animal to predict that the blood osmolarities can return to normal than the animal stops drinking beautiful.

It's just beautiful. The brain is essentially predicting with it sounds like a high degree of accuracy how much water one needs to drink linking it to the pleasure and of ingesting good clean water under conditions where we're thirsty in anticipation of adjusting blood osmolarity in 20 minutes. Exactly. I mean it's um yeah I mean this is the kind of thing that just it delights me because it just means that the brain as a predictive organ is just is so accurate.

It also explains some some sort of funny aspects of thirst that you may have noticed from every day experience so. So you know one one idea is that just cooling your mouth can sort of quench your thirst right so if you're in the hospital and you're not allowed to drink any fluids they'll give you ice chips to suck on the sort of quench your thirst so why is that.

And so one idea is that perhaps because water is usually cooler than your body that sensation of water pass it always cools your mouth and so you learn or maybe it's an eight that just cooling of my mouth means that basically I'm going to be rehydrated.

So Chris this is an experiment by gratitude and Chris Zimmerman Chris did the same thing where he was recording these thirst neurons put a cold piece of metal on the mouse's tongue and you can see when you do that these thirst neurons go down in activity and then you remove the cold piece of metal and they go back up amazing. So a lot of these these sort of oddities of everyday experience have to do with how the system is evolved to make the prediction about what's going to happen to the body.

I mean few things are as rewarding as the sensation of drinking really nice clean cold water when one is very thirsty when my lab was in San Diego I used to take my dog. I came Palomar mountain in one day you know I really screwed up he was bulldog mastiff they overheat easily and it was a lot warmer than we thought we ran out of water it was actually dangerous situation for him.

We got down to the bottom of the hill thankfully with him still alive and there's this pump that pumps what is I was told was spring water and it came out you know really cold and you could just see him fill back up with life.

Yes I feel back up with life known he was filling back up with life and it was was unlike that the kind of reward that one experiences with food when you're hungry absolutely that basic critical need for water absolutely under conditions where you're clearly dehydrated is like nothing else it's delicious in a way that no food is delicious I would actually say something about so so that distinction you made is really interesting between hunger and thirst.

So when you stimulate these neurons that make an animal thirsty the mice hate it they will do anything to avoid something that artificially makes them thirsty so we can artificially stimulate these are create a virtual thirst. They'll lever press hundreds of times to make it stop the same neurons that the neurons I talked about the control hunger the H.R. P neurons they actually don't care so much they won't really do much of anything to shut them off.

That raises the question why do they animals eat them when you stimulate the hunger neurons and we think the primary thing that the hunger neurons stimulation does is it make food itself more attractive it makes the food more delicious more of an attractive motivational magnet it makes the experience of eating more pleasurable but it is not itself the most unpleasant state at least the mice aren't going to do that much or as for thirst I think you know dehydration in thirst is really just unpleasant and animals just want to avoid that and so I think that distinction is is very real I think there are two different motivational mechanisms.

For hunger and thirst hunger is mostly about the reward of food thirst is mostly about this is just really unpleasant and removing that unpleasant exactly and you had a paper which I was going to ask you about so I will entitled the four brain thirst circuit drives drinking through negative reinforcement yes and I'm guessing that paper illustrates exactly the point is made so it's a four brain circuit so it does that mean that there's some elements of learning and cognition around this or we broadly speaking about the four brain for instance the hypothalamus

being in the four brain so yeah it's interesting so so the third circuit for whatever reason is mostly in the four brain so the neurons that so we talked about the NTS in the area post-stream being important for hunger and signals signals from the God those are the eight area post-stream is a circumventricular organ meeting a south side the blood brain barrier is only a couple of these in the brain the neurons that control thirst are located in the two circumventricular organs in the four brain one is called the sub four-nacle organ the other one is called the OVLT

but they're just acronyms but so why it evolved to have the thirst neurons more in the four brain and the the neurons that sense nutrients more in the high brain is a little bit unclear and so there is definitely an element of learning but a lot of this is those neurons are also just directly sensing the blood and sensing changes in in both the concentration of salt in the blood and then also hormones like angiotensin the triphthirst

I was going to ask you this earlier but it seems appropriate to us now a colleague of mine at Stanford in the psychology department Dr. Ali Krum who studies mindsets has done some interesting experiments where people are told that a given milkshake is

colorically dense other people are told that a milkshake is colorically sparse both groups independently consume the milkshake and then they measure things like hormone responses in the bloodstream that are associated with satiety and what she finds is that

even hormone responses to the same shake meaning the same amount of calories fat sugar etc can be significantly modulated based on what we're told and it extends into some other perhaps even more interesting areas in my opinion whereby if people are told that

let's say a given meal that has a small piece of fish serving of vegetables and a carburetidrate is yes perhaps a little bit colorically sparse compared to what would normally eat at a given meal but they're told this is a highly nutritious meal this is good for you then just that mere knowledge

can drive more satiety better feelings about the meal even I believe I have to double check on this but as I recall a heightened sense of of it tasting really good so humans are very susceptible to the in this case the either inaccurate in the case of the milkshake experiment or accurate descriptions of food meaning they shape our our perception of whether or not something is good for us tastes good or not and whether or not it leads to more or less satiety and I think this is important

given the obesity crisis you know to say nothing of these drugs that are coming out whereby people often associate dieting with deprivation and pain but if they understand that certain foods are nutritious that can at least partially offset some of the pain of caloric restriction what are your thoughts on on that yeah well so one thing I've been talking about is how a lot of these circuits are anticipatory they're making predictions they're trying to estimate what's happening in the future

and I talked about how these age therapy hunger neurons how they can sort of see the food or get input about the the side smell of food and that way predict how many calories the mouse is going to eat but I mean this is a mouse right there's all based on a mouse and mouse has you know a thousand

times fewer neurons than you do as a person right so the computational capacity that the human brain has to make these predictions is just vast compared to these and these mice are already doing amazing things right so when you think about then what is the human brain able to do in terms of

anticipating changes in traditional state and how information that you're given can change the expected physiologic outcomes I mean you're right I mean there's there's there's just this whole other element that it's very hard to study because it's happening in the brains of humans if we can't do these kinds of experiments but I'm sure that's very important I mean so I talked a little bit about about these flavor nutrient conditioning experiments these are the experiments where essentially

an animal learns to consume a certain flavor because it learns it's going to be associated with nutrients later sort of the paradigm for how you learn to consume bitter vegetables because they're good for you and you get nutrients so people've also done those experiments in humans and that

does work but what they've discovered is it's very sensitive to what you tell the humans about the thing that they're going to consume so if you put nutritional labels where you show the different numbers of calories then basically they sort of adjust their expectations and nothing happens it

really has to be that sort of it's very sensitive to what information you give them before the experiment happens so I think that's an example of that kind of thing without any pressure for it to be prescriptive how do you approach eating given the knowledge that you have about food

I like to assume that you can sit down to a meal and not think about your agr p neurons too much or any of that but given that you have deep knowledge in this has it shaped kind of how you think about food cravings your own you don't have to real with those are even if they exist how you

observe the eating behavior of others and yeah what how is knowledge shaped your feeding behavior well I try not to think too much about my agr p neurons when I'm eating because I would hope I would hope I think it gets I think you know the circuitry is so complex and we're just beginning to see

what's happening so I wouldn't I wouldn't use that kind of information at this stage in there we're just beginning to prescriptively but I think I think there is a set of you know basic recommendations from physiology and neuroscience very simple things you probably talked about

people on your podcast before for sort of shaping your diet to be healthier to limit food and take so one we've already talked about is limiting consumption of ultra processed food eating more whole foods for lots of different reasons because it's they're more satiating because they don't

have this sort of engineered palatability that causes you to overeat another big one which I'm sure you've talked about with some of your guests is protein consumption making sure you get adequate protein consumption both because there's this concept of protein leveraging so if you don't eat a minimum amount of protein that's going to cause you to eat more calories just to try to achieve that minimum amount of protein also just because proteins more satiating and also because there's this

idea of thermic effective food and so you basically burn more calories metabolizing protein than sugar or fat how about consumption of fluids during meals you know you I've heard it said before that um you know we're not supposed to consume too many fluids because it's going to dilute the enzymes that that allows to digest our food I've heard other people say that's complete um I think that's myth I've never that's a myth I think I mean I think um drinking water I mean so humans don't have a

perfect capacity to determine whether they're hungry or thirsty and so drinking water will ensure you're not eating because you're hungry because you're thirsty um and uh so and there's there's no idea of diluting it I don't think that man you know distension itself even though when the water

provides a very limited distension signal the expansion of your stomach and intestines is one important way interesting that you that you terminate feeding and so and so there is some component of that where you can get distension just from drinking water I say as I have blurted out interesting

because I didn't realize that a fluid consumption um only provides a limited signal for distension not fluids it's water and so the idea is that that you can fill your stomach up with fluids but the rate at which fluids empty out of your stomach depends on their calorie content so basically if you

drink water it empties very rapidly into your intestine and then it goes through your intestine and is gradually absorbed if you drink something like glass of orange juice it will empty much more slowly and if you drink something that's really high in fat really high in calories it'll empty

extremely slowly over hours and that's because there's a negative feedback loop from the intestine that controls gastric emptying so as those first nutrients leave your um uh stomach and enter your intestine that produces hormones that go back and then slow down the rate of gastric emptying and

the purpose for this is that you don't want nutrients entering the intestine too fast that's really unsafe it feels very unpleasant and uh it's just your intestine can only metabolize nutrients so fast and so if there's calories then it slows down and gastric emptying a lot but water just kind

of goes through more beautiful system like there's regulation at every point right hypothalamus brainstem got a rate of emptying based on the difference between water and orange juice it's just awesome yeah and this part of the reason i think it's so hard to outsmart the system right because you know these you know these neurons are making predictions based on the sight and smell of food but then the god is doing its own thing that's calculating it separately and relaying that information

so at every step there are these checks basically they're just confirming the what you thought happened the first time it's actually what's really going on and so and it's which makes sense because it's

so important for survival these homeostatic systems are the product you know so much natural selection which i think at least partially explains why thousand fold increases in peptide hormones like GLP1 are required to see significant long lasting changes in weight exactly um because the system is

so strongly regulated exactly exactly it's hard to beat homeostasis and hard to beat it safely but it sounds like you're more or less optimistic about where that whole field of of um let's call it anti obesity drugs is headed i'm very optimistic i mean i think look i mean i think that it's you

couldn't have asked for more so far at this stage with these with these GLP1 drugs i'm incredible weight loss unexpected health benefits really safe as far as we can tell i mean there is always possible that some new uh some new side effect will emerge but these drugs are millions of people

and they've been in a lot of people for a long time now and nothing seems to have shown up so um i'm very optimistic and i think even beyond that just now that the pharmaceutical industry is reinvigorated to investigate this question there's so many different people working out in five

years people have so many different options it won't just be ozampic or margaro there will be five different ten different drugs that they can choose from um that have slightly different side effect profiles slightly different efficacy perhaps used for people with slightly different

metabolic conditions um and so it really be a whole palette of of medicines you can take that will uh adjust your physiology and hunger and it's amazing how well it um squares with the understanding of the basic biology you know and um and that's a perfect opportunity for me to

really just say what uh is in my mind and clearly in the minds of everyone listening and and watching which is thank you so much for this absolutely encyclopedic and exceptionally clear explanation of feeding and thirst and salt regulation and these new drugs that are you know

in everyone's minds and everyone's hearing about um i've learned so much today i know everyone else has uh you run a uh incredible laboratory i've tracked your career for a very long time every paper is is spectacular and you're in a very competitive field and you've contributed in

enormous ways to our understanding of these important processes and i don't just say that as a formality i know that to be true given that we you know um are from the same field and have uh known each other for a long time and i'm familiar with your work at at at a deep level um

today has just been an absolute privilege and a gift to learn from you and um i know everyone feels the same way so thank you for taking time out of your busy research schedule and the other important areas of your life to come here and educate us all i learned so much basic and practical

knowledge and i know uh everyone else did as well thank you so much thank you this has been really fun i'm really glad we had a chance to do this we talked about some of my favorite topics so it's always a pleasure and talk with another neuroscientist about these things is fantastic so well please come back again um meanwhile thanks for everything you do all right thanks thank you for joining me for

today's discussion with dr. Zachary Knight to learn more about his research or to support his laboratories work please see the links in the show note captions if you're learning from end or enjoying this podcast please subscribe to our youtube channel please also subscribe to the podcast

on both Spotify and apple by clicking the follow button that's a terrific zero cost way to support us also on both Spotify and apple you can leave us up to a five star review please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode that's the best way to

support this podcast if you have questions for me or comments about the podcasts or topics or guests you'd like me to consider for the huberman lab podcast please put those in the comment section on youtube i do read all the comments if you're not already following me on social media i am huberman lab on all social media platforms so that's instagram x formerly known as twitter threads facebook and linkedin and on all those channels i cover science and science related tools some of which overlap

with the contents of the huberman lab podcast but much of which is distinct from the contents on the huberman lab podcast so again that's huberman lab on all social media platforms and if you haven't already subscribed to our neural network newsletter our neural network newsletter is a

zero cost monthly newsletter that includes podcast summaries as well as brief one to three page pdf's of what we call protocols those protocols cover everything from learning and neuroplasticity to dopamine regulation to improving your sleep we also have a foundational fitness protocol the details

cardiovascular training and resistance training sessions right down to the details again all of that is available completely zero cost you simply go to huberman lab calm go to the menu tabs scroll down to newsletter and enter your email and i should point out that we do not share your email with anybody thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion with dr. zackry night and last but certainly not least thank you for your interest in science

This transcript was generated by Metacast using AI and may contain inaccuracies. Learn more about transcripts.