When Did Scientists Hide Under Beds to Do Research? - podcast episode cover

When Did Scientists Hide Under Beds to Do Research?

Jan 19, 20246 min
--:--
--:--
Listen in podcast apps:

Episode description

In the early 1900s, psychologists went to great lengths to study their subjects without letting them know they were being watched. Learn more about their research (and about how ethical standards would prevent it from happening today) in this episode of BrainStuff, based on this article: https://history.howstuffworks.com/historical-events/ridiculous-history-when-scientists-hid-beds-do-research.htm

See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.

Transcript

Speaker 1

Welcome to Brainstuff, a production of iHeartRadio. Hey, Brainstuff. Lauren Vogelbaum here, it's quite a conundrum needing to conduct research on people who don't know your conducting research on them. After all, when people know they're being watched, they may very well behave differently than they otherwise would. This is a scientific predicament as old as the science of psychology.

But today we're talking about researchers Mary Henley and Marian B. Hubble and a study that they did in the nineteen thirties. By the way, Henley went on to be an important expert in the stalt psychology. And I tried to look up how she pronounced her name but couldn't find it. Henla is the traditional German way of saying it, but it's often Henlee in English, so I'm going with that

at any rate. As part of Henley's psychology graduate work at britainmar College, a women's school in Pennsylvania, Henley and Hubble were trying to determine whether children become less ego centric as they grow older. In order for the researchers to get a real feel for the conversations of college students, they took any means necessary, but to quote their paper Egocentricity and Adult Conversation, published in the Journal of Social

Psychology in May of nineteen thirty eight. In order not to introduce artifacts into the conversations, the investigators took special precautions to keep the students ignorant of the fact that their remarks were being recorded. They concealed themselves under beds in students rooms where tea parties were being held, eavesdropped in dormitory smoking rooms and dormitory washrooms, and listened to telephone conversations. And the researchers didn't confine themselves just to

students campus activities. They also captured remarks in waiting rooms, hotel lobbies, theaters, and restaurants, even on the street car. They pursued their unsuspecting subjects in the streets, in department stores, and into their homes. In each case, the researchers jotted down a verbatim record of the remarks on the scene before the article. This episode is based on how Stfforks

spoke about email doctor Ali Mattou, a clinical psychologist. He explained the hallmark of psychological science is experimentation, highly controlling an environment and only manipulating one experimental variable. While this type of research can tell us a lot about the relationship between cause and effect, experimental studies can sometimes lack external validity, which is to say, the more control a researcher exerts on an experiment, the less it seems like

real life. Observational studies like the one Henley and Hubble carried out pose a way to mitigate that effect, although they have their own drawbacks. The duo hoped to get good data without the biasing effect of letting their subjects know they were being observed. Because again, knowing you're being observed changes your behavior, Mattu said, this is called objective self awareness. This can be helped full in a lot

of situations. Banks and other high security environments show you camera security footage of yourself to trigger objective self awareness and reduce the chances you might do something stupid for the purposes of research. Knowing that you are being observed can lead to reactivity. People might act the way they think an experimenter wants them to act, or they might act more in line with cultural expectations. They might also act the opposite of what is expected because they know

that this is an artificial situation back in Henley. In Hubble's time, though the concept of objective self awareness hadn't been defined, they also lacked something else critical to research studies today, informed consent that would arise from the Nuremberg Code, created after World War II, an important set of ethical standards for the treatment of human subjects and scientific experiments of all kinds, upon which many international regulations and guidelines

have been based. Mattus said. The rules now state that people must be fully aware of an experiment's risks and benefits before they sign on to participate. Additionally, you cannot involve anyone in any type of research without their complete consent, and if at any time they want to pull out of a study, they can. Scientists could do a sort of modern take on Henley and Hubble's experiment today, but

in a much more controlled and etical way. All research has to go through institutional review boards that work to protect participants from studies that would have scientists hiding under their beds. Mattu said. Researchers can easily study behavior in public spaces without getting informed consent from others, as long as they don't reveal any identifying information about the people being observed. Researchers typically do this by showing results in aggregate.

For example, someone studying public behavior in times square could describe how many times people help each other out, as long as they don't describe specifics of individuals, a meaning that if the authors of the nineteen thirty eight study wanted to get their results today, their methodology would need

to be drastically different to conform to ethical standards. In case you're curious, Henley and Hubble found that adults don't shed their egocentricity after all, and they've had a lot of time to think about it, hiding under beds and sneaking around campus. Today's episode is based on the article Ridiculous History when scientists hit under beds to do research on HowStuffWorks dot com, written by Brian young a brain Stuff is production of iHeartRadio in partnership with hastuffworks dot

Com and is produced by Tyler Klang. Four more podcasts from my heart Radio visit the iHeartRadio app, Apple Podcasts, or wherever you listen to your favorite shows.

Transcript source: Provided by creator in RSS feed: download file